Part of
Language in Interaction: Studies in honor of Eve V. Clark
Edited by Inbal Arnon, Marisa Casillas, Chigusa Kurumada and Bruno Estigarribia
[Trends in Language Acquisition Research 12] 2014
► pp. 207230
References
Akhtar, N., & Montague, L
(1999) Early lexical acquisition: The role of cross-situational learning. First Language , 19, 347–358. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baker, C., Saxe, R., & Tenenbaum, J
(2009) Action understanding as inverse planning. Cognition , 113, 329–349. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baldwin, D
(1993) Early referential understanding: Infants’ ability to recognize referential acts for what they are. Developmental Psychology , 29, 832–843. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1995) Understanding the link between joint attention and language. In C. Moore & P. Dunham (Eds.), Joint attention: Its origins and role in development (pp. 131–158). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Bergelson, E., & Swingley, D
(2012) At 6–9 months, human infants know the meanings of many common nouns. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences , 109, 3253–3258. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bloom, P
(2002)  How children learn the meanings of words . Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Brown, P., Pietra, V., Pietra, S., & Mercer, R
(1993) The mathematics of statistical machine translation: Parameter estimation. Computational Linguistics , 19, 263–311.Google Scholar
Callanan, M
(1985) How parents label objects for young children: The role of input in the acquisition of category hierarchies. Child Development , 56, 508–523. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Carpenter, M., Nagell, K., & Tomasello, M
(1998) Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development , 63(4/Serial no. 255), 1–174. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, E.V
(1988) On the logic of contrast. Journal of Child Language , 15, 317–335. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1990) On the pragmatics of contrast. Journal of Child Language , 17, 417–431. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2003)  First language acquisition . Cambridge: CUP.Google Scholar
Clark, H.H
(1996)  Using language . Cambridge: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Csibra, G., & Gergely, G
(2009) Natural pedagogy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences , 13, 148–153. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fazly, A., Alishahi, A., & Stevenson, S
(2010) A probabilistic computational model of cross-situational word learning. Cognitive Science , 34, 1017–1063. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fisher, C., Hall, D., Rakowitz, S., & Gleitman, L
(1994) When it is better to receive than to give: Syntactic and conceptual constraints on vocabulary growth. Lingua , 92, 333–375. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Frank, M.C., & Gibson, E
(2011) Overcoming memory limitations in rule learning. Language Learning and Development , 7, 130–148. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Frank, M.C., Goldwater, S., Griffiths, T.L., & Tenenbaum, J.B
(2010) Modeling human performance in statistical word segmentation. Cognition , 117, 107–125. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Frank, M.C., & Goodman, N.D
(2012) Predicting pragmatic reasoning in language games. Science , 336, 998. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Under review). Inferring word meanings by assuming that speakers are informative.
Frank, M.C., Goodman, N.D., Lai, P., & Tenenbaum, J.B
(2009) Informative communication in word production and word learning. In N. Taatgen & H. van Rijn (Eds.), Proceedings of the 31st Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society , (pp. 1228–1233). Amsterdam: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Frank, M.C., Goodman, N.D., & Tenenbaum, J.B
(2007) A Bayesian framework for cross-­situational word learning. Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems , 20.Google Scholar
(2009) Using speakers’ referential intentions to model early cross-situational word learning. Psychological Science , 20, 578–585. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Frank, M.C., Tenenbaum, J.B., & Fernald, A
(2013) Social and discourse contributions to the determination of reference in cross-situational word learning. Language Learning and Development , 9, 1–24. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gallistel, C
(1990)  The organization of learning . Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Gallistel, C., Fairhurst, S., & Balsam, P
(2004) The learning curve: Implications of a quantitative analysis. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences , 101, 13124–13131. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Geisler, W
(2003) Ideal observer analysis. In L.M. Chalupa & J.S. Werner (Eds.), The visual neurosciences (pp. 825–837). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Gergely, G., Bekkering, H., & Király, I
(2002) Rational imitation in preverbal infants. Nature , 415, 755. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gillette, J., Gleitman, H., Gleitman, L., & Lederer, A
(1999) Human simulations of vocabulary learning. Cognition , 73, 135–176. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gleitman, L
(1990) The structural sources of verb meanings. Language Acquisition , 1, 3–55. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goodman, N.D., & Stuhlmüller, A
(2013) Knowledge and implicature: Modeling language understanding as social cognition. Topics in Cognitive Science , 5, 173–184. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gopnik, A
(2012) Scientific thinking in young children. Theoretical advances, empirical research and policy implications. Science , 337, 1623–1627. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grice, H
(1975) Logic and conversation. Syntax and Semantics , 3, 41–58.Google Scholar
Griffin, Z., & Bock, K
(2000) What the eyes say about speaking. Psychological Science , 11, 274–279. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hollich, G., Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Golinkoff, R
(2000) Breaking the language barrier: An emergentist coalition model for the origins of word learning. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development , 65, 1–135. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
James, W
(1890)  The principles of psychology , Vol. 1. New York, NY: Henry Holt and Company. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Johnson, M., Demuth, K., & Frank, M
(2012) Exploiting social information in grounded language learning via grammatical reductions. In Proceedings of the Association for Computational Linguistics (pp. 883–891).
Johnson, M., Griffiths, T., & Goldwater, S
(2007) Adaptor grammars: A framework for specifying compositional nonparametric Bayesian models. Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems , 19, 641–648.Google Scholar
Li, P., Farkas, I., & MacWhinney, B
(2004) Early lexical development in a self-organizing neural network. Neural Networks , 17, 1345–1362. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Locke, J
(1690/1964)  An essay concerning human understanding . Cleveland, OH: Meridian Books.Google Scholar
Markman, E.M
(1991)  Categorization and naming in children: Problems of induction . Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Marr, D
(1982)  Vision: A computational investigation into the human representation and processing of visual information . New York, NY: Henry Holt & Co.Google Scholar
McMurray, B., Horst, J.S., & Samuelson, L.K
(2012) Word learning emerges from the interaction of online referent selection and slow associative learning. Psychological Review , 119, 831–877. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Medina, T., Snedeker, J., Trueswell, J., & Gleitman, L
(2011) How words can and cannot be learned by observation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences , 108, 9014–9019. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mitchell, C., & McMurray, B
(2009) On leveraged learning in lexical acquisition and its relationship to acceleration. Cognitive Science , 33, 1503–1523. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Onishi, K., & Baillargeon, R
(2005) Do 15-month-old infants understand false beliefs? Science , 308, 255–258. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Perfors, A., Tenenbaum, J., Griffiths, T., & Xu, F
(2011) A tutorial introduction to bayesian models of cognitive development. Cognition , 120, 302–321. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pinker, S
(1984)  Language learnability and language development . Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Quine, W
(1960)  Word and object . Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Regier, T
(2005) The emergence of words: Attentional learning in form and meaning. Cognitive Science , 29, 819–865. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sanborn, A., Griffiths, T., & Navarro, D
(2010) Rational approximations to rational models: alternative algorithms for category learning. Psychological Review , 117, 1144. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schmidt, L., Goodman, N.D., Barner, D., & Tenenbaum, J
(2009) How tall is tall? Compositionality, statistics, and gradable adjectives. In N. Taatgen & H. van Rijn (Eds.), Proceedings of the 31st Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society , (pp. 3151–3156).
Schulz, L., Kushnir, T., & Gopnik, A
(2007) Learning from doing: Intervention and causal inference. In A. Gopnik & L. Schulz (Eds.), Causal learning: Psychology, philosophy, and computation (pp. 67–85). Oxford: OUP.Google Scholar
Shafto, P., Goodman, N.D., & Frank, M
(2012) Learning from others the consequences of psychological reasoning for human learning. Perspectives on Psychological Science , 7, 341–351. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Siskind, J
(1996) A computational study of cross-situational techniques for learning word-to-meaning mappings. Cognition , 61, 39–91. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Smith, L.B., & Yu, C
(2008) Infants rapidly learn word-referent mappings via cross-situational statistics. Cognition , 106, 1558–1568. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
St. Augustine
(397/1963)  The confessions of St. Augustine . Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Stiller, A., Goodman, N.D., & Frank, M
(2011) Ad-hoc scalar implicature in adults and children. In L. Carlson, C. Hoelscher, & T.F. Shipley (Eds.), Proceedings of the 33rd Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society , (pp. 2134–2139).
Tenenbaum, J., & Griffiths, T
(2001) Generalization, similarity, and Bayesian inference. Behavioral and Brain Sciences , 24, 629–640.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M
(2003)  Constructing a language: A usage-based theory of language acquisition . Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Trueswell, J.C., Medina, T.N., Hafri, A., & Gleitman, L.R
(2013) Propose but verify: Fast mapping meets cross-situational word learning. Cognitive Psychology , 66, 126–156. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vouloumanos, A
(2008) Fine-grained sensitivity to statistical information in adult word learning. Cognition , 107, 729–742. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vouloumanos, A., Onishi, K., & Pogue, A
(2012) Twelve-month-old infants recognize that speech can communicate unobservable intentions. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences , 109, 12933–12937. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vouloumanos, A., & Werker, J
(2009) Infants’ learning of novel words in a stochastic environment. Developmental Psychology , 45, 1611–1617. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Waxman, S., & Booth, A
(2001) Seeing pink elephants: Fourteen-month-olds’ interpretations of novel nouns and adjectives. Cognitive Psychology , 43, 217–242. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yu, C., & Ballard, D
(2007) A unified model of early word learning: Integrating statistical and social cues. Neurocomputing , 70, 2149–2165. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yu, C., & Smith, L
(2012) Modeling cross-situational word–referent learning: Prior questions. Psychological Review , 119, 21. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 1 other publications

Lee, Crystal & Chigusa Kurumada
2021. Learning Maximum Absolute Meaning Through Reasoning About Speaker Intentions. Language Learning 71:2  pp. 326 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 12 april 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.