Article published in:
Input and Experience in Bilingual Development
Edited by Theres Grüter and Johanne Paradis
[Trends in Language Acquisition Research 13] 2014
► pp. 1536
References

References

Bedore, L.M., Peña, E.D., Summers, C.L., Boerger, K.M., Resendiz, M.D., Greene, K., Bohman, T.M., & Gillam, R.B.
(2012) The measure matters: Language dominance profiles across measures in Spanish–English bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15, 616–629. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
De Houwer, A.
(2011) Language input environments and language development in bilingual acquisition. Applied Linguistics Review, 2, 221-240. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dunn, L.M., & Dunn, L.M.
(1997) Peabody picture vocabulary test – Third edition (PPVT–III). Circle Pines, MN: AGS Publishing.Google Scholar
Dunn, L.M., Padilla, E., Lugo, D., & Dunn, L.M.
(1986) Test de vocabulario en imágenes Peabody (TVIP). Circle Pines, MN: AGS Publishing.Google Scholar
Elin Thordardottir, Rothenberg, A., Rivard, M-E., & Naves, R.
(2006) Bilingual assessment: Can overall proficiency be estimated from separate measurement of two languages? Journal of Multilingual Communication Disorders, 4, 1 – 21. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Elin Thordardottir
(2011) The relationship between bilingual exposure and vocabulary development. International Journal of Bilingualism, 15, 426-445. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fenson, L., Marchman, V.A., Thal, D., Dale, P.S., Reznick, J.S., & Bates, E.
(2007) MacArthur-Bates communicative development inventories: User’s guide and technical manual (2nd ed.). Baltimore, MD: Brookes.Google Scholar
Fernald, A., & Marchman, V.A.
(2012) Individual differences in lexical processing at 18 months predict vocabulary growth in typically-developing and late-talking toddlers. Child Development, 83, 203-222. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fernald, A., Perfors, A., & Marchman, V.A.
(2006) Picking up speed in understanding: Speech processing efficiency and vocabulary growth across the second year. Developmental Psychology, 42, 98-116. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fernald, A., Pinto, J.P., Swingley, D., Weinberg, A., & McRoberts, G.W.
(1998) Rapid gains in speed of verbal processing by infants in the second year. Psychological Science, 9, 72-75. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fernald, A., Zangl, R., Portillo, A.L., & Marchman, V.A.
(2008) Looking while listening: Using eye movements to monitor spoken language comprehension by infants and young children. In I. Sekerina, E.M. Fernández, & H. Clahsen (Eds.), Developmental psycholinguistics: On-line methods in children’s language processing (pp. 97-135). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, V.C.M., & Thomas, E.M.
(2009) Bilingual first-language development: Dominant language takeover, threatened minority language take-up. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 12, 213-237. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gutiérrez-Clellen, V.F., & Kreiter, J.
(2003) Understanding child bilingual acquisition using parent and teacher reports. Applied Psycholinguistics, 24, 267-288. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hart, B., & Risley, T.R.
(1995) Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.Google Scholar
Hoff-Ginsberg, E.
(1992) How should frequency in input be measured? First Language, 12, 233-244. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hoff, E.
(2003) The specificity of environmental influence: Socioeconomic status affects early vocabulary development via maternal speech. Child Development, 74, 1368-1878. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2006) How social contexts support and shape language development. Developmental Review, 26, 55-88. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hoff, E., Core, C., Place, S., Rumiche, R., Señor, M., & Parra, M.
(2012) Dual language exposure and early bilingual development. Journal of Child Language, 39, 1-27. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hurtado, N., Grüter, T., Marchman, V.A., & Fernald, A.
(2014) Relative language exposure, processing efficiency and vocabulary in Spanish–English bilingual toddlers. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17, 189-202. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hurtado, N., Marchman, V.A., & Fernald, A.
(2007) Spoken word recognition by Latino children learning Spanish as their first language. Journal of Child Language, 33, 27-249.Google Scholar
(2008) Does input influence uptake? Links between maternal talk, processing speed and vocabulary size in Spanish-learning children. Developmental Science, 11, F31-F39. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Huttenlocher, J., Haight, W., Bryk, A., Seltzer, M., & Lyons, T.
(1991) Vocabulary growth: Relation to language input and gender. Developmental Psychology, 27, 236-248. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Jackson-Maldonado, D., Thal, D.J., Marchman, V.A., Newton, T., Fenson, L., & Conboy, B.
(2003) MacArthur inventarios del desarrollo de habilidades comunicativas: User’s guide and technical manual. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.Google Scholar
Marchman, V.A., & Fernald, A.
(2008) Speed of word recognition and vocabulary knowledge in infancy predict cognitive and language outcomes in later childhood. Developmental Science, 11, F9–F16. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Marchman, V.A., Fernald, A., & Hurtado, N.
(2010) How vocabulary size in two languages relates to efficiency in spoken word recognition by young Spanish–English bilinguals. Journal of Child Language, 37, 817–840. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2013) Early spoken language understanding predicts language and cognitive outcomes in Spanish-speaking toddlers. Manuscript submitted for publication.Google Scholar
Marchman, V.A., Martínez, L., Hurtado, N., Grüter, T., & Fernald, A.
2014). Absolute amounts of caregiver talk in each language matter more than relative exposure for young Spanish-English bilinguals. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Marchman, V.A., Martínez-Sussmann, C., & Dale, P.S.
(2004) The language-specific nature of grammatical development: Evidence from bilingual language learners. Developmental Science, 7, 212-224. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Newport, E.L., Gleitman, H., & Gleitman, L.R.
(1977) Mother, I’d rather do it myself: Some effects and non-effects of maternal speech style. In C. Snow & C.A. Ferguson (Eds.), Talking to children: Language input and acquisition (pp. 109-150). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Paradis, J., & Genesee, F.
(1996) Syntactic acquisition in bilingual children: Autonomous or independent? Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 18, 1–25. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Patterson, J.
(1998) Expressive vocabulary development and word combinations of Spanish–English bilingual toddlers. American Journal of Speech–Language Pathology, 7, 46–56. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pearson, B.Z., Fernández, S.C., Lewedeg, V., & Oller, D.K.
(1997) The relation of input factors to lexical learning by bilingual infants. Applied Psycholinguistics, 18, 41-58. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Place, S., & Hoff, E.
(2011) Properties of dual language exposure that influence 2-year-olds’ bilingual proficiency. Child Development, 82, 1834-1849. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tanenhaus, M., & Trueswell, J.
(1995) Sentence comprehension. In J.L. Miller & P.D. Eimas (Eds.), Speech, language and communication: Handbook of perception and cognition (2nd ed., pp. 217-262). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Vasilyeva, M., Waterfall, H., & Huttenlocher, J.
(2008) Emergence of syntax: Commonalities and differences across children. Developmental Science, 11, 84–97. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Weisleder, A., & Fernald, A.
(2013) Talking to children matters: Early language experience strengthens processing and builds vocabulary. Psychological Science, 24, 2143-2152. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 4 other publications

Core, Cynthia
2020.  In Child Bilingualism and Second Language Learning [Bilingual Processing and Acquisition, 10],  pp. 87 ff. Crossref logo
Ortega, Lourdes
2020. The Study of Heritage Language Development From a Bilingualism and Social Justice Perspective. Language Learning 70:S1  pp. 15 ff. Crossref logo
Sun, He, Marc H. Bornstein & Gianluca Esposito
2021. The Specificity Principle in Young Dual Language Learners’ English Development. Child Development 92:5  pp. 1752 ff. Crossref logo
Valentini, Alessandra & Ludovica Serratrice
2021. What Can Bilingual Children Tell Us About the Developmental Relationship Between Vocabulary and Grammar?. Cognitive Science 45:11 Crossref logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 19 november 2021. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.