Article published in:
Acquisition and Development of Hebrew: From infancy to adolescence
Edited by Ruth A. Berman
[Trends in Language Acquisition Research 19] 2016
► pp. 201224
References

References

Abbot-Smith, K. & Tomasello, M.
2006Exemplar-learning and schematization in a usage based account of syntactic acquisition. The Linguistic Review 23: 275–290. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Albert, A., MacWhinney, B., Nir, B. & Wintner, S.
2013The Hebrew CHILDES corpus: Transcription and morphological analysis. Language Resources and Evaluation 47(4), 973–1005. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Armon-Lotem, S. & Berman, R.
2003The emergence of grammar: Early verbs and beyond. Journal of Child Language 30(4): 845–877. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Arnon, I.
2010Starting Big: The Role of Multiword Phrases in Language Learning and Use. PhD dissertation, Stanford University.Google Scholar
Arnon, I. & Snider, N.
2010More than words: Frequency effects for multi-word phrases. Journal of Memory and Language 62(1): 67–82. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Arnon, I. & Clark, E.V.
2011Why brush your teeth is better than teeth – Children’s word production is facilitated in familiar sentence-frames. Language Learning and Development 72: 107–129. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Arnon, I. & Cohen Priva, U.
2013More than words: The effect of multi-word frequency and constituency on phonetic duration. Language and Speech 56(3): 349–371. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
2014Time and again: The changing effect of word and multiword frequency on phonetic duration for highly frequent sequences. The Mental Lexicon 9(3): 377-400. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bannard, C. & Matthews, D.
2008Stored word sequences in language learning: The effect of familiarity on children’s repetition of four-word combinations. Psychological Science 19(3): 241–8. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bannard, C., Lieven, E. & Tomasello, M.
2009Modeling children’s early grammatical knowledge. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 106(41): 17284–9. Crossref
Berman, R.
1978Modern Hebrew Structure. Tel Aviv: University Publication Project.Google Scholar
1985The acquisition of Hebrew. In The Crosslinguistic Study of Language Acquisition D.I. Slobin (ed.), 255–371. Hillsdale NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
1990On acquiring an (S)VO language: Subjectless sentences in children’s Hebrew. Linguistics 286: 1135–1166.Google Scholar
Berman, R.A.
(2002) Crosslinguistic comparisons in later language development. In The Diversity of Languages and Language Learning. S. Strömqvist (ed.), 25-44. Lund: Center for Languages and Literature.Google Scholar
Berman, R. & Ravid, D.
2000Research in acquisition of Israeli Hebrew and Palestinian Arabic. Hebrew Studies 41: 83–98. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Berman, R. & Lustigman, L.
2014Emergent clause-combining in adult-child interactional contexts. In Language in Interaction. Studies in Honor of Eve V. Clark [Trends in Language Acquisition Research 12], I. Arnon, M. Casillas, C. Kurumada, & B. Estigarribia (eds) 281–302. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Biber, D.
2009A corpus-driven approach to formulaic language in English: Multi-word patterns in speech and writing. International Journal of Corpus Linguistics 14(3): 275–311. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bod, R.
2009From exemplar to grammar: A probabilistic analogy-based model of language learning. Cognitive Science 335: 752–93. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bracha Nir, B.M.S.W.
2010A Morphologically-Analyzed CHILDES Corpus of Hebrew. LREC . http://​citeseerx​.ist​.psu​.edu​/viewdoc​/summary​?doi​=10​.1​.1​.178​.8392
Bybee, J.
1998The Emergent Lexicon. In The 34th Chicago Linguistic Society CLS 34: The panels, 421-435. Chicago IL: CLS.Google Scholar
Cameron-Faulkner, T., Lieven, E. & Tomasello, M.
2003A construction based analysis of child directed speech. Cognitive Science 27(6): 843–873. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N.
1965Aspects of the Theory of Syntax. Cambridge MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Culicover, P.W. & Jackendoff, R.
2005Simpler Syntax. Oxford: OUP. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Da̧browska, E. & Lieven, E.
2005Towards a lexically specific grammar of children’s question constructions. Cognitive Linguistics 16(3): 437–474. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Diessel, H.
2007Frequency effects in language acquisition, language use, and diachronic change. New Ideas in Psychology 252: 108–127. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Elman, J.
2009On the meaning of words and dinosaur bones: Lexical knowledge without a lexicon. Cognitive Science 33: 547–582. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Givón, T.
1973Complex NP’s, resumptive pronouns, and word order in Hebrew. In Papers from the Ninth Regional Meeting of the Chicago Linguistic Society, C.T. Corum, T., C. Smith-Stark & A. Wieser (eds),135–146. Chicago IL: CLS.Google Scholar
1976On the VS order in Israeli Hebrew: Pragmatics and typological change. In Studies in Modern Hebrew Syntax and Semantics, P. Cole (ed.), 153–181. Amsterdam: North-Holland.Google Scholar
Goldberg, A.
2006Constructions at Work. Oxford: OUP.Google Scholar
Huttenlocher, J., Vasilyeva, M., Cymerman, E. & Levine, S.
2002Language input and child syntax. Cognitive Psychology 45(3): 337–374. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kirjavainen, M., Theakston, A. & Lieven, E.
2009Can input explain children’s me-for-I errors? Journal of Child Language 36(5): 1091–114. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Langacker, R.W.
1987Foundations of Cognitive Grammar: Theoretical Prerequisites. Stanford CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Lieven, E.V.M., Pine, J.M. & Baldwin, G.
1997Lexically-based learning and early grammatical development. Journal of Child Language 24(1): 187–219. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lieven, E., Behrens, H., Speares, J. & Tomasello, M.
2003Early syntactic creativity: A usage-based approach. Journal of Child Language 30(2): 333–370. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M.
2008Children’s first language acquisition from a usage-based perspective. In, Handbook of Cognitive Linguistics and Second Language Acquisition, P. Robinson & N.C. Ellis (eds), 168–196. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Lieven, E., Salomo, D. & Tomasello M.
2009Two-year-old children's production of multiword utterances: A usage-based analysis. Cognitive Linguistics 20(3): 481-508. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lieven, E.
2010Input and first language acquisition: Evaluating the role of frequency. Lingua 120(11): 2546–2556. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
2014First language development: A usage-based perspective on past and current research. Journal of Child Language 41(Suppl. 1): 48–63. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
MacWhinney, B.
2000The CHILDES Project: Tools For Analyzing Talk. Mahwah NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
McClelland, J.L.
2010Emergence in cognitive science. Topics in Cognitive Science 2(4): 751–770. http://​philpapers​.org​/rec​/MCCEIC DOI: CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Mintz, T.H.
2003Frequent frames as a cue for grammatical categories in child directed speech. Cognition 90(1): 91–117. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Naigles, L.R. & Hoff-Ginsburg, E.
1998Why are some verbs learned before other verbs? Effects of input frequency and structure on children’s early verb use. Journal of Child Language 25(1): 95–120. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Peters, A.M.
1977Language learning strategies : Does the whole equal the sum of the parts ? Language 53(3): 560–573. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
1983The Units of Language Acquisition. Cambridge: CUP.Google Scholar
Pinker, S.
1999Words and Rules: The Ingredients of Language. New York NY: HarperCollinsGoogle Scholar
Ramscar, M., Yarlett, D., Dye, M., Denny, K. & Thorpe, K.
2010The effects of feature-label-order and their implications for symbolic learning. Cognitive Science 34(6): 909–957. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ramscar, M.
(2013) Suffixing, prefixing, and the functional order of regularities in meaningful strings. Psihologija 46(4): 377–396. http://​www​.doiserbia​.nb​.rs​/Article​.aspx​?ID​=0048​-57051304377R​&AspxAutoDetectCookieSupport​=1#​.VPnUArOUe5I DOI: CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ravid, D., Dressler, W.U., Nir, B., Korecky-Kröll, K., Souman, A., Rehfeldt, K., Laaha, S., Bertl, J., Basbøll, H. & Gillis, S.
2009Core morphology in child directed speech: Crosslinguistic corpus analyses of noun plurals. In Corpora in Language Acquisition Research: History, methods, perspectives [Trends in Language Acquisition Research 6], H. Behrens (ed.), 149–162. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Reali, F. & Christiansen, M.H.
2007Word chunk frequencies affect the processing of pronominal object-relative clauses. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (2006) 60(2): 161–70. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rowland, C.F. & Pine, J.M.
2000Subject–auxiliary inversion errors and wh-question acquisition: “What children do know?” Journal of Child Language 27(01): 157–181. http://​journals​.cambridge​.org​/abstract​_S0305000999004055 DOI: CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Seidl, A. & Johnson, E.K.
2006Infant word segmentation revisited: Edge alignment facilitates target extraction. Developmental Science 9(6): 565–573. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Stoll, S., Abbot-Smith, K. & Lieven, E.
2009Lexically restricted utterances in Russian, German, and English child-directed speech. Cognitive Science 33(1): 75–103. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Theakston, A.L, Lieven, E.V.M, Pine, J.M. & Rowland, C.F.
2001The role of performance limitations in the acquisition of verb-argument structure: an alternative account. Journal of Child Language 28: 127-152. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M.
2003Constructing a Language: A Usage-Based Theory of Language Acquisition. Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Tremblay, A., Derwing, B., Libben, G. & Westbury, C.
2011Processing advantages of lexical bundles: Evidence from self-paced reading and sentence recall tasks. Language Learning 61(2): 569–613. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Uziel-Karl, S.
2001A Multidimensional Perspective on the Acquisition of Verb Argument Structure. Tel-Aviv: Tel-Aviv University.Google Scholar
2006Acquisition of verb structure from a developmental perspective: Evidence from child Hebrew. In The Acquisition of Verbs and their Grammar: The Effect of Particular Languages, N. Gagarina & I. Gülzow (eds), 33: 15–44. Dordrecht: Springer. CrossrefGoogle Scholar