Part of
Sources of Variation in First Language Acquisition: Languages, contexts, and learners
Edited by Maya Hickmann †, Edy Veneziano and Harriet Jisa
[Trends in Language Acquisition Research 22] 2018
► pp. 123138
References (51)
References
Allen, S., Özyürek, A., Kita, S., Brown, A., Furman, R., Ishizuka, T. & Fujii, M. 2007. Language-specific and universal influences in children’s packaging of manner and path: A comparison of English, Japanese, and Turkish. Cognition 102(1): 16–48. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bates, E. 1976. Language and Context: The Acquisition of Pragmatics. New York NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bates, E., Benigni, L., Bretherton, I., Camaioni, L. & Volterra, V. 1979. The Emergence of Symbols: Cognition-Communication in Infancy. New York NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Brown, P. 2008. Verb specificity and argument realization in Tzeltal child language. In Crosslinguistic Perspectives on Argument Structure, M. Bowerman & P. Brown, 167–189. New York NY: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Cardini, F. 2010. Evidence against Whorfian effects in motion conceptualization. Journal of Pragmatics 42(5): 1442–1459. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cartmill, E. A., Demir, Ö.E. & Goldin-Meadow, S. 2012. Studying gesture. In Research Methods in Child Language: A Practical Guide, E. Hoff (ed.), 208–225. Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Choi, S. & Bowerman, M. 1991. Learning to express motion events in English and Korean: The influence of language-specific lexicalization patterns. Cognition 41(1–3): 83–121. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Colletta, J., Guidetti, M., Capirci, O., Cristilli, C., Demir, O., Kunene-Nicolas, R. & Levine, S. 2014. Effects of age and language on co-speech gesture production: an investigation of French, American, and Italian children’s narratives. Journal of Child Language 42(1): 122–145. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Demir, Ö. E., So, W. -C., Özyürek, A. & Goldin-Meadow, S. 2012. Turkish- and English-speaking children display sensitivity to perceptual context in the referring expressions they produce in speech and gesture. Language and Cognitive Processes 27(6): 844–867. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Furman, R. 2012. Caused Motion Events in Turkish: Verbal and Gestural Representation in Adults and Children. PhD dissertation, Radboud University Nijmegen.Google Scholar
Furman, R., Kuntay, A. & Özyürek, A. 2014. Early language-specificity of children’s event encoding in speech and gesture: Evidence from caused motion in Turkish. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience 29: 620–634. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gentner, D. 1982. Why are nouns learned before verbs: Linguistic relativity versus natural partitioning. In Language Development: Language, Thought and Culture, Vol. 2, S. A. Kuczaj (ed.), 301–334. Hillsdale NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S. & Alibali, M. W. 2013. Gesture’s role in speaking, learning, and creating language. Annual Review of Psychology 123: 448–453. DOI logo.Google Scholar
Göksun, T., Hirsh-Pasek, K. & Golinkoff, R. M. 2010. How do preschoolers express cause in gesture and speech? Cognitive Development 25(1): 56–68. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Greenfield, P. & Smith, J. 1976. The Structure of Communication in Early Language Development. New York NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Gu, Y., Mol, L., Hoetjes, M. & Swerts, M. 2014. Does language shape the production and perception of gestures? A study on late Chinese-English bilinguals’ conceptions about time. In Proceedings of the 36th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, P. Bello, M. Guarini, M. McShane & B. Scassellati (eds). <[URL]> (19 December 2016).
Gullberg, M. 2011. Language-specific encoding of placement events in gestures. In Event Representation in Language and Cognition, J. Bohnemeyer & E. Pederson (eds), 166–188. Cambridge: CUP.Google Scholar
Gullberg, M., Hendricks, H. & Hickmann, M. 2008. Learning to talk and gesture about motion in French. First Language 28(2): 200–36. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gullberg, M. & Narasimhan, B. 2010. What gestures reveal about how semantic distinctions develop in Dutch children’s placement verbs. Cognitive Linguistics 21(2): 239–262. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hickmann, M., Taranne, P. & Bonnet, P. 2009. Motion in first language acquisition: Manner and path in French and English child language. Journal of Child Language 36(4): 705–741. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hostetter, A. B. & Alibali, M. W. 2008. Visible embodiment: Gestures as simulated action. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 15: 495–514. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Iverson, J. M., Capirci, O. & Caselli, M. C. 1994. From communication to language in two modalities. Cognitive Development 9(1): 23–43. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Iverson, J. M., Capirci, O., Volterra, V. & Goldin-Meadow, S. 2008. Learning to talk in a gesture-rich world: Early communication in Italian vs. American children. First Language 28(2): 164–181. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Iverson, J. M. & Goldin-Meadow, S. 2005. Gesture paves the way for language development. Psychological Science 16(5): 367–371. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kendon, A. 2004. Gesture: Visible Action as Utterance. Cambridge: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ketrez, N. & Aksu-Koc, A. 2002. Acquisition of noun and verb categories in Turkish. In Studies in Turkish Linguistics: Proceedings of the Xth International Conference on Turkish Linguistics, A. S. Özsoy, D. Akar, M. Nakipoğlu-Demiralp, E. Erguvanlı-Taylan & A. Koc (eds), 239–246. Istanbul: Boğaziçi University PressGoogle Scholar
Kita, S. 2009. Cross-cultural variation of speech-accompanying gesture: A review. Language and Cognitive Processes 24(2): 145–167. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kita, S. & Özyürek, A. 2003. What does cross-linguistic variation in semantic coordination of speech and gesture reveal? Evidence for an interface representation of spatial thinking and speaking. Journal of Memory and Language 48 (1): 16–32. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kita, S., Özyürek, A., Allen, S., Brown, A., Furman, R. & Ishizuka, T. 2007. Relations between syntactic encoding and co-speech gestures: Implications for a model of speech and gesture production. Language and Cognitive Processes, 22(8): 1212–1236. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Küntay, A. & Slobin, D. I. 1996. Listening to a Turkish mother: Some puzzles for acquisition. In Social Interaction, Social Context, and Language, D. I. Slobin, J. Gerhardt, A. Kyratzis & J. Guo (eds), 265–286. Mahwah NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Liszkowski, U., Brown, P., Callaghan, T., Takada, A. & De Vos, C. 2012. A prelinguistic gestural universal of human communication. Cognitive Science 36(4): 698–713. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McNeill, D. 1992. Hand and Mind. Chicago IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Namy, L. L., Campbell, A. L. & Tomasello, M. 2004. The changing role of iconicity in non-verbal symbol learning: A U-shaped trajectory in the acquisition of arbitrary gestures. Journal of Cognition and Development 5: 37–57. (Special issue). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nicoladis, E. 2002. Some gestures develop in conjunction with spoken language development and others don’t: Evidence from bilingual preschoolers. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior 26(4): 241–266. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nicoladis, E., Mayberry, R. I. & Genesee, F. 1999. Gesture and early bilingual development. Developmental Psychology 35(2): 514–526. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Oh, K. 2003. Language, Cognition and Development: Motion Events in English and Korean. PhD dissertation, University of California Berkeley.Google Scholar
Özçalışkan, Ş. 2007. Metaphors we move by: Children’s developing understanding of metaphorical motion in typologically distinct languages. Metaphor & Symbol 22(2): 147–68. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2012. When gesture does and does not follow speech in describing motion. In Online supplement for the Proceedings of the 36th Boston University Conference on Language Development, A. Biller, E. Chung & A. Kimball (eds). <[URL]> (19 December 2016).
Özçalışkan, Ş., Gentner, D. & Goldin-Meadow, S. 2014. Do iconic gestures pave the way for children’s early verbs? Applied Psycholinguistics 35: 1143–1162. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Özçalışkan, Ş. & Goldin-Meadow, S. 2005. Gesture is at the cutting edge of early language development. Cognition 96(3): B101–B113. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2009. When gesture-speech combinations do and do not index linguistic change. Language and Cognitive Processes 24(2): 190–217. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2011. Is there an iconic gesture spurt at 26 months? In Integrating Gestures: The Interdisciplinary Nature of Gesture [Gesture Studies 4], G. Stam & M. Ishino (eds), 163–174. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Özçalışkan, Ş. & Slobin, D. I. 1999. Learning ‘how to search for the frog’: Expression of manner of motion in English, Spanish and Turkish. In Proceedings of the 23rd Boston University Conference on Language Development, A. Greenhill, H. Littlefield & C. Tano (eds), 541–552. Somerville MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Özyürek, A., Furman, R. & Goldin-Meadow, S. 2014. On the way to language: Event segmentation in homesign and gesture. Journal of Child Language 42: 64–94. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Özyürek, A., Kita, S., Allen, S., Furman, R. & Brown, A. 2005. How does linguistic framing of events influence co-speech gestures? Insights from crosslinguistic variations and similarities. Gesture 5(1–2): 219–240. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Özyürek, A., Kita, S., Allen, S., Furman, R., Brown, A. & Ishizuka, T. 2008. Development of cross-linguistic variation in speech and gesture: Motion events in English and Turkish. Developmental Psychology 44(4): 1040–1054. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Papafragou, A., Massey, C. & Gleitman, L. 2002. Shake, rattle, 'n' roll: The representation of motion in language and cognition. Cognition 84: 189–219. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2006. When English proposes what Greek presupposes: The cross-linguistic encoding of motion events. Cognition 98: B75–87. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rowe, M. & Goldin-Meadow, S. G. 2009. Differences in early gesture explain SES disparities in child vocabulary size at school entry. Science 323: 951–953. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Talmy, L. 1985. Lexicalization patterns: Semantic structure in lexical forms. In Language Typology and Semantic Description, Vol. III: Grammatical Categories and the Lexicon, T. Shopen (ed.), 36–149. Cambridge: CUP.Google Scholar
2000. Toward a Cognitive Semantics: Vol. II: Typology and Process in Concept Structuring. Cambridge MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Cited by (4)

Cited by four other publications

KARADÖLLER, Dilay Z., Beyza SÜMER, Ercenur ÜNAL & Aslı ÖZYÜREK
2024. Sign advantage: Both children and adults’ spatial expressions in sign are more informative than those in speech and gestures combined. Journal of Child Language 51:4  pp. 876 ff. DOI logo
Mittelberg, Irene & Jennifer Hinnell
2024. Iconicity, Schematicity, and Representation in Gesture. In The Cambridge Handbook of Gesture Studies,  pp. 56 ff. DOI logo
Brown, Lucien, Hyunji Kim, Iris Hübscher & Bodo Winter
2022. Gestures are modulated by social context. Gesture 21:2-3  pp. 167 ff. DOI logo
Karadöller, Dilay Z.
2022. Development of spatial language and memory. Sign Language & Linguistics 25:2  pp. 235 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 28 september 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.