Part of
Sources of Variation in First Language Acquisition: Languages, contexts, and learners
Edited by Maya Hickmann †, Edy Veneziano and Harriet Jisa
[Trends in Language Acquisition Research 22] 2018
► pp. 339364
References

References

Acredolo, L. & Goodwyn S.
1988Symbolic gesturing in normal infants. Child Development 59(2): 450–466. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Anderson, D. & Reilly, J.
1997The puzzle of negation : How children move from communicative to grammatical negation in ASL., Journal of Child Language 18(4): 411–429.Google Scholar
Arbib, M. A.
2012How the Brain Got Language: The Mirror System Hypothesis. Oxford: OUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bates, E., Benigni, L., Bretherton, I., Camaioni, L. & Volterra, V.
1979The Emergence of Symbols: Cognition and Communication in Infancy. New York NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Beaupoil-Hourdel, P.
2015Acquisition et expression multimodale de la négation. Etude d’un corpus vidéo et longitudinal de dyades mère-enfant francophone et anglophone. PhD dissertation, Université Sorbonne Nouvelle – Paris 3.Google Scholar
Beaupoil-Hourdel, P., Boutet, D. & Morgenstern, A.
2015A child’s multimodal negations from 1 to 4: The interplay between modalities. In Negation and Polarity: Experimental Perspectives, Language, Cognition, and Mind, P. Larrivée & C. Lee (eds), 95–123. Bern: Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bellugi, U.
1967A Transformational Analysis of the Development of Negation. PhD dissertation, Harvard University.Google Scholar
Benazzo, S. & Morgenstern, A.
2014A bilingual child’s multimodal path into negation. Gesture 14(2): 171–202. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Blondel, M.
2009Acquisition bilingue LSF-français. L'enfant qui grandit avec deux langues et dans deux modalités. In Au croisement de différents types d'acquisition: Pourquoi et comment comparer?, S. Benazzo (ed.). AILE…LIA 1: 169–194.Google Scholar
Bloom, L.
1970Language Development: Form and Function in Emerging Grammars. Cambridge MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
1991Language Development from Two to Three. Cambridge: CUP.Google Scholar
Butcher, C. & Goldin-Meadow, S.
2000Gesture and the transition from one- to two-word speech: When hand and mouth come together. In Language and Gesture, D. McNeill (ed.), 235–257. Cambridge: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Caët, S.
2013Référence à soi et à l’interlocuteur chez des enfants francophones et anglophones et leurs parents. PhD dissertation, Université Sorbonne Nouvelle – Paris 3.Google Scholar
Calbris, G.
2005La négation: Son symbolisme physique. Interacting Bodies – Corps en Interaction. Second Conference of the International Society for Gesture Studies, Lyon: France.Google Scholar
Cameron-Faulkner, T., Lieven, E. & Theakston, A. L.
2007What part of no do children not understand? A usage-based account of multiword negation. Journal of Child Language 33: 251–282. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Capirci O., Iverson, J. M., Pizzuto, E. & Volterra, V.
1996Gestures and words during the transition to two-word speech. Journal of Child Language 23(03): 645–673. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Choi, S.
1988The semantic development of negation: A cross-linguistic study. Journal of Child Language 15: 517–532. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, E. V.
1978From gesture to word, on the natural history of deixis in language acquisition. In Human Growth and Development: Wolfson College Lectures 1976, J. S. Bruner & A. Garton (eds), 85–120. Oxford: OUP.Google Scholar
Clark, H. H. & Clark, E. V.
1977Psychology of Language: An Introduction to Psycholinguistics. New York NY: Harcourt, Brace.Google Scholar
Darwin, C.
1872The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals. London: John Murray. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dodane, C. & Massini-Cagliari, G.
2010La prosodie dans l’acquisition de la négation : étude de cas d’une enfant monolingue française. ALFA: Revista de Linguística, 54(2).Google Scholar
Ekman, P. & Friesen, W.V.
1969The repertoire or nonverbal behavior: categories, origins, usage, and coding. Semiotica 1: 49–98.Google Scholar
Emmorey, K.
2002Language, Cognition, and the Brain: Insights from SIGN Language Research. Hillsdale NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Guidetti, M.
2005Yes or no? How do young children combine gestures and words to agree and refuse? Journal of child language 32: 911–924. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Harrison, S.
Horn, L. R.
2001A Natural History of Negation. Stanford CA: CSLI.Google Scholar
Kanto, L., Lassako, M., Huttunen, K.
2015Differentiation in language and gesture use during early bilingual development of hearing children of deaf parents. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 18(4): 769–788. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kendon, A.
1988How gestures can become like words. In Cross-cultural Perspectives in Nonverbal Communication, F. Poyatos (ed.), 131–141. Lewiston NY: C.J. Hogrefe.Google Scholar
2002Some uses of the headshake. Gesture 2(2): 147–182. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2004Gesture: Visible Action as Utterance. Cambridge: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kita, S. & Özyürek, A.
2003What does cross-linguistic variation in semantic coordination of speech and gesture reveal? Evidence for an interface representation of spatial thinking and speaking. Journal of Memory and Language 48: 16–32. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Limousin, F.
2011Acquisition de la reference personnelle en LSF: Analyse longitudinale des pointages, des forms nulls et des noms signés chez une enfant soured de parents sourds. PhD dissertation, University of Paris 8.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B.
2000The CHILDES Project: Tools for analyzing talk. Transcription format and programs. Vol. 1. Routledge.Google Scholar
Mayberry, R. & Nicoladis, E.
2000Gesture reflects language development: Evidence from bilingual children. Psychological science 9(6): 192–196.Google Scholar
McNeill, D.
1992Hand and Mind: What Gestures Reveal about Thought. Chicago IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
McNeill, D. & McNeill. N. B.
1968What does a child mean when he says no? In Proceedings of the Conference on Language and Language Behaviour, E. M. Zale (ed.), 464–473. New York NY: Appleton Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
Morgenstern, A.
2006Un JE en construction. Paris: Presses de la Sorbonne Nouvelle.Google Scholar
2009L’enfant dans la langue. Paris: Presses de la Sorbonne Nouvelle.Google Scholar
Morgenstern, A. & Beaupoil, P.
2015Multimodal approaches to language acquisition through the lens of negation. Vestnik of Moscow State University. Linguistics and Literary Studies 6(717): 435–451. Special issue on Discourse as Social Practice.Google Scholar
Morgenstern, A., Beaupoil, P., Blondel, M. & Boutet, D.
2016A multimodal approach to the development of negation in signed and spoken languages: Four case studies. In The Usage-Based Study of Language Learning and Multilingualism, L. Ortega, A. E. Tyler, H. I. Park & M. Uno (eds), 15–36. Washington DC: Georgetown University Press.Google Scholar
Morgenstern, A. & Parisse, C.
2012The Paris Corpus. French Language Studies 22(1): 7–12. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Morgenstern, A., Leroy, M. & Caët S.
2013Self- and other-repairs in child-adult interaction at the intersection of pragmatic abilities and language acquisition. Journal of Pragmatics 56: 151–167. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nicoladis, E.
2007The effect of bilingualism on the use of manual gestures. Applied Psycholinguistics 28: 441–454. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nicoladis, E., Mayberry R. J. & Genesee, F.
1999Gesture and early bilingual development. Developmental Psychology 35(2): 514–526. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kimbrough Oller, D. & Jarmulowicz, L.
2007Language and literacy in bilingual children in the early school years. In Handbook of Language Development, E. Hoff & M. Shatz (eds), 368–386. Oxford: Blackwell. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Parisse, C. & Morgenstern, A.
2012The unfolding of the verbal temporal system in 2 French children’s speech between 18 and 36 months. French Language Studies 22(1): 95–114. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pea, R. D.
1980The development of negation in early child language. In The Social Foundations of Language and Thought: Essays in Honor of Jerome Seymour Bruner, D. R. Olson (ed.), 156–186. New York NY: W.W. Norton.Google Scholar
Sekali, M.
2012The emergence of complex sentences in a French child’s language from 0;10 to 4;01: Causal adverbial clauses and the concertina effect. Journal of French Language Studies 22: 115–141. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shaw, E.
2013Gesture in Multiparty Interaction: A Study of Embodied Discourse in Spoken English and American Sign Language. PhD dissertation, Georgetown University.Google Scholar
Spitz, R. A.
1957No and Yes: On the Genesis of Human Communication. New York NY: International Universities Press.Google Scholar
Stern, C. & Stern, W.
1907[1928]Die Kindersprache – Eine psychologische Untersuchung. Leipzig: Barth.Google Scholar
Streeck, J.
2009Gesturecraft: The Manu-facture of Meaning [Gesture Studies 2]. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sutton-Spence, R. & Woll, B.
1999The Linguistics of British Sign Language. Cambridge: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, Michael
2003Constructing a Language: A Usage-Based Theory of Language Acquisition. Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Tuller L., Blondel, M. & Niederberger N.
2007Growing up in French and French Sign Language. In French Applied Linguistics [Language Learnng and Language Teaching 16], D. Ayoun (ed.), 334–376. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vaidyanathan, R.
1991Development of forms and functions of negation in the early stages of language acquisition: A study in Tamil. Journal of Child Language 18: 51–66. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Volterra, V. & Antinucci, F.
1979Negation in child language: A pragmatic study. In Developmental pragmatics, E. Ochs & B. B. Schieffelin (eds), 281–303. New York NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Volterra, V., Caselli, M. C., Capirci, O., Pizzuto, E.
2004Gesture and the emergence and development of language. In Beyond Nature-Nurture: Essays in Honor of Elisabeth Bates, M. Tomasello & D. I. Slobin (eds), 22–44. Mahwah NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Wode, H.
1977Four early stages in the development of L1 negation. Journal of Child Language 4: 87–102. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zeshan, U.
2006Negative and interrogative constructions in sign languages: A case study in sign language typology. In Interrogative and Negative Constructions in Sign Languages [Sign Language Typology 1], U. Zeshan (ed.), 28–68. Nijmegen: Ishara Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zlatev, J., Persson T. & Gärdenfors, P.
2005Bodily mimesis as “the missing link”. In Human Cognitive Evolution [LUCS 121]. Lund: Lund University Cognitive Studies.Google Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 4 other publications

Benazzo, Sandra & Aliyah Morgenstern
2014. A bilingual child’s multimodal path into negation. Gesture 14:2  pp. 171 ff. DOI logo
Boutet, Dominique, Marion Blondel, Pauline Beaupoil-Hourdel & Aliyah Morgenstern
2021. A multimodal and kinesiological approach to the development of negation in signing and non-signing children. Languages and Modalities 1  pp. 31 ff. DOI logo
Harrison, Simon & Silva H. Ladewig
Morgenstern, Aliyah, Lea Chevrefils, Marion Blondel, Coralie Vincent, Chloé Thomas, Jean-François Jego & Dominique Boutet
2021. “Of thee I sing”: An opening to Dominique Boutet’s kinesiological approach to gesture. Languages and Modalities 1  pp. 3 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 9 may 2023. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.