Part of
The Development of Prosody in First Language Acquisition
Edited by Pilar Prieto and Núria Esteve-Gibert
[Trends in Language Acquisition Research 23] 2018
► pp. 1735
References
Allopenna, P. D., Magnuson, J. S., & Tanenhaus, M. K.
(1998) Tracking the time course of spoken word recognition using eye movements: Evidence for continuous mapping models. Journal of Memory and Language, 439(38), 419–439. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Armstrong, M. E., Andreu, L., Esteve-Gibert, N., & Prieto, P.
(2016) Children’s processing of morphosyntactic and prosodic cues in overriding context-based hypotheses: An eye tracking study. Probus, 28(1), 57–90.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bernard, C., & Gervain, J.
(2012) Prosodic cues to word order: What level of representation? Frontiers in Psychology, 3(October), 451.Google Scholar
Bortfeld, H., Morgan, J. L., Golinkoff, R. M., & Rathbun, K.
(2005) Mommy and me: Familiar names help launch babies into speech-stream segmentation. Psychological Science, 16(4), 298–304. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Choi, Y., & Mazuka, R.
(2003) Young children’s use of prosody in sentence parsing. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 32(2), 197–217. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Christophe, A., Mehler, J., & Sebastián-Gallés, N.
(2001) Perception of prosodic boundary correlates by newborn infants. Infancy, 2(3), 385–394. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Christophe, A., Millotte, S., Bernal, S., & Lidz, J.
(2008) Bootstrapping lexical and syntactic acquisition. Language and Speech, 51(1–2), 61–75. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Christophe, A., Nespor, M., Guasti, M. T., & Van Ooyen, B.
(2003) Prosodic structure and syntactic acquisition: The case of the head-direction parameter. Developmental Science, 6(2), 211–220. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Christophe, A., Peperkamp, S., Pallier, C., Block, E., & Mehler, J.
(2004) Phonological phrase boundaries constrain lexical access I. Adult data. Journal of Memory and Language, 51(4), 523–547. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cutler, A., & Butterfield, S.
(1992) Rhythmic cues to speech segmentation: Evidence from juncture misperception. Journal of Memory and Language, 31(2), 218–236. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
de Carvalho, A., Dautriche, I., Lin, I., & Christophe, A.
(2017) Phrasal prosody constrains syntactic analysis in toddlers. Cognition, 163, 67–79.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
de Carvalho, A., Dautriche, I., & Christophe, A.
(2016) Preschoolers use phrasal prosody online to constrain syntactic analysis. Developmental Science, 19(2), 235–250.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
de Carvalho, A., He, X. A., Lidz, J., & Christophe, A
(2015) 18-month-olds use the relationship between prosodic and syntactic structures to constrain the meaning of novel words. Paper presented at the 40th Boston University Conference on Language Development, Boston, USA.Google Scholar
de Carvalho, A., Lidz, J., Tieu, L., Bleam, T., & Christophe, A.
(2016) English-speaking preschoolers can use phrasal prosody for syntactic parsing. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 139(6), EL216–EL222.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Decasper, A., & Spence, M.
(1986) Prenatal maternal speech influences newborns’ perception of speech sounds. Infant Behavior and Development, 9, 133–150. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dryer, M.
(1992) The greenbergian word order correlations. Language, 68(1), 81–138. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Echols, C. H.
(1993) A perceptually-based model of children’s earliest productions. Cognition, 46, 245–296. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Echols, C. H., & Newport, E.
(1992) The role of stress and position in determining first words. Language Acquisition, 2, 189–220. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Endress, A. D., & Hauser, M. D.
(2010) Word segmentation with universal prosodic cues. Cognitive Psychology, 61(2), 177–199. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gerken, L., Jusczyk, P. W., & Mandel, D. R.
(1994) When prosody fails to cue syntactic structure 9-month-olds’ sensitivity to phonological versus syntactic phrases. Cognition, 51, 237–265. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gervain, J., Nespor, M., Mazuka, R., Horie, R., & Mehler, J.
(2008) Bootstrapping word order in prelexical infants: A Japanese-Italian cross-linguistic study. Cognitive Psychology, 57(1), 56–74. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gervain, J., & Werker, J. F.
(2013) Prosody cues word order in 7-month-old bilingual infants. Nature Communications, 4, 1490. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gleitman, L.
(1990) The structural sources of verb meanings. Language Acquisition, 1, 3–55. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gout, A., Christophe, A., & Morgan, J. L.
(2004) Phonological phrase boundaries constrain lexical access II. Infant data. Journal of Memory and Language, 51(4), 548–567. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hawthorne, K., & Gerken, L.
(2014) From pauses to clauses: Prosody facilitates learning of syntactic constituency. Cognition, 133(2), 420–428. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hawthorne, K., Mazuka, R., & Gerken, L.
(2015) The acoustic salience of prosody trumps infants’ acquired knowledge of language-specific prosodic patterns. Journal of Memory and Language, 82, 105–117. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
He, A. X., & Lidz, J.
(2017) Verb learning in 14- and 18-month-old English-learning infants. Language Learning and Development, 5441(March).Google Scholar
Hirschberg, J., & Pierrehumbert, J.
(1986) The intonational structuring of discourse. Proceedings of the 24th Meeting of the Association for Computational Linguistics– New York, Vol. 1, 136–144.DOI logo
Hirsh-Pasek, K., Kemler Nelson, D. G., Jusczyk, P. W., Cassidy, K. W., Druss, B., & Kennedy, L.
(1987) Clauses are perceptual units for young infants. Cognition, 26, 269–286. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Höhle, B., Weissenborn, J., Schmitz, M., & Ischebeck, A.
(2001) Discovering word order regularities: The role of prosodic information for early parameter setting. In J. Weissenborn, & B. Höhle (Eds.), Approaches to bootstrapping: Phonological, lexical, syntactic and neurophysiological aspects of early language acquisition (pp.246–263). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Johnson, E. K.
(2008) Infants use prosodically conditioned acoustic-phonetic cues to extract words from speech. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 123(6), EL144–EL148. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Johnson, E. K., & Seidl, A.
(2008) Clause segmentation by 6-month-old infants: A crosslinguistic perspective. Infancy, 13(5), 440–455. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jun, S.
(2005) Prosodic typology. In S. Jun (Ed.), Prosodic typology: The phonology of intonation and phrasing (pp.430–458). Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2014) Prosodic typology: By prominence type, word prosody, and macro-rhythm. In S. Jun (Ed.), Prosodic typology II: The phonology of intonation and phrasing (pp.520–540). Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jusczyk, P. W.
(1997) The discovery of spoken language. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Jusczyk, P. W., Houston, D. M., & Newsome, M.
(1999) The beginnings of word segmentation in english-learning infants. Cognitive Psychology, 39(3–4), 159–207. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kjelgaard, M. M., & Speer, S. R.
(1999) Prosodic facilitation and interference in the resolution of temporary syntactic closure ambiguity. Journal of Memory and Language, 40(2), 153–194. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ladd, R.
(2008) Intonational phonology. Lingua (2nd ed., Vol. 104). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lew-Williams, C., Pelucchi, B., & Saffran, J. R.
(2011) Isolated words enhance statistical language learning in infancy. Developmental Science, 14(6), 1323–1329. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mandel, D. R., Jusczyk, P. W., & Nelson, D. G.
(1994) Does sentential prosody help infants organize and remember speech information? Cognition, 53(2), 155–180. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Männel, C., & Friederici, A. D.
(2009) Pauses and intonational phrasing: ERP studies in 5-month-old German infants and adults. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(10), 1988–2006. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Maris, E., & Oostenveld, R.
(2007) Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG- and MEG-data. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 164(1), 177–190. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Massicotte-Laforge, S., & Shi, R.
(2015) The role of prosody in infants’ early syntactic analysis and grammatical categorization. Journal of Acoustical Society of America, 138(October), 441–446. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mehler, J., Jusczyk, P., Lamsertz, G., Halsted, N., Bertoncini, J., & Amiel-Tison, C.
(1988) A precursor of language acquisition in young infants. Cogntion, 29, 143–178. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Michelas, A., & D’Imperio, M.
(2015) Prosodic boundary strength guides syntactic parsing of French utterances. Laboratory Phonology, 6(1), 119–146. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Millotte, S., René, A., Wales, R., & Christophe, A.
(2008) Phonological phrase boundaries constrain the online syntactic analysis of spoken sentences. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34(4), 874–885. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Millotte, S., Wales, R., & Christophe, A.
(2007) Phrasal prosody disambiguates syntax. Language and Cognitive Processes, 22(6), 898–909. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Millotte, S., Morgan, J., Margules, S., Bernal, S., Dutat, M., & Christophe, A.
(2010) Phrasal prosody constrains word segmentaion in French 16-month-olds. Journal of Portuguese Linguistics, 9–2(10–1), 67–86.Google Scholar
Morgan, J.
(1986) From Simple Input to Complex Grammar. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Morgan, J., & Demuth, K.
(1996) Signal to syntax: Bootstrapping from speech to grammar in early acquisition. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Nazzi, T., Bertoncini, J., & Mehler, J.
(1998) Language discrimination by newborns: Toward an understanding of the role of rhythm. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 24(3), 756–766. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nazzi, T., Iakimova, G., Bertoncini, J., Frédonie, S., & Alcantara, C.
(2006) Early segmentation of fluent speech by infants acquiring French: Emerging evidence for crosslinguistic differences. Journal of Memory and Language, 54, 283–299. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nespor, M., Shukla, M., van de Vijver, R., Avesani, C., Schraudolf, H., & Donati, C.
(2008) Different phrasal prominence realization in VO and OV languages. Lingue e Linguaggio, 2, 1–29.Google Scholar
Nespor, M., & Vogel, I.
(1986) Prosodic phonology. Dordrecht: Foris.Google Scholar
Pierrehumbert, J. B., & Hirschberg, J.
(1990) The meaning of intonational contours in the interpretation of discourse. In P. Cohen, J. Morgan, & M. Pollack (Eds.) Intentions in communication (pp.271–311). Cambridge, MA: Bradford Books, The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Saffran, J., Aslin, R., & Newport, E.
(1996) Statistical learning by 8-month-old infants. Science, 274(5294), 1926–1928. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Seidl, A.
(2007) Infants’ use and weighting of prosodic cues in clause segmentation. Journal of Memory and Language, 57(1), 24–48. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shattuck-Hufnagel, S., & Turk, E.
(1996) A prosody tutorial for investigators of auditory sentence processing. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 25(2), 193–247. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shi, R., Werker, J. F., & Morgan, J. L.
(1999) Newborn infants’ sensitivity to perceptual cues to lexical and grammatical words. Cognition, 72(2), B11–B21. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shukla, M., Nespor, M., & Mehler, J.
(2007) An interaction between prosody and statistics in the segmentation of fluent speech. Cognitive Psychology, 54(1), 1–32. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shukla, M., White, K. S., & Aslin, R. N.
(2011) Prosody guides the rapid mapping of auditory word forms onto visual objects in 6-mo-old infants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(15), 6038–6043. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Snedeker, J., & Trueswell, J
(2001) Unheeded cues: Prosody and syntactic ambiguity in mother-child communication. Paper presented at the 26th Boston University Conference on Language Development,Boston, USA.Google Scholar
Snedeker, J., & Trueswell, J.
(2003) Using prosody to avoid ambiguity: Effects of speaker awareness and referential context. Journal of Memory and Language, 48, 103–130. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Snedeker, J., & Yuan, S.
(2008) Effects of prosodic and lexical constraints on parsing in young children (and adults). Journal of Memory and Language, 58, 574–608. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Soderstrom, M., Seidl, A., Nelson, D. G., & Jusczyk, P. W.
(2003) The prosodic bootstrapping of phrases: Evidence from prelinguistic infants. Journal of Memory and Language, 49(2), 249–267. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vogel, I., & Raimy, E.
(2002) The acquisition of compound vs. phrasal stress: the role of prosodic constituents. Journal of Child Language, 29(2), 225–250. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Warner, N., Otake, T., & Arai, T.
(2010) Intonational structure as a word-boundary cue in Tokyo Japanese. Language and Speech, 53(1), 107–131. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Weber, A., Grice, M., & Crocker, M. W.
(2006) The role of prosody in the interpretation of structural ambiguities: a study of anticipatory eye movements. Cognition, 99(2), B63–B72. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wellmann, C., Holzgrefe, J., Truckenbrodt, H., Wartenburger, I., & Höhle, B.
(2012) How each prosodic boundary cue matters: Evidence from German infants. Frontiers in Psychology, 3(DEC), 1–13.Google Scholar
Werker, J. F., Cohen, L., Lloyd, V., & Casasola, M.
(1998) Acquisition of word-object associations by 14-month-old infants. Developmental Psychology, 34(6), 1289–1309. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zhou, P., Crain, S., & Zhan, L.
(2012) Sometimes children are as good as adults: The pragmatic use of prosody in children’s on-line sentence processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 67(1), 149–164. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 9 other publications

Benítez‐Burraco, Antonio & Wendy Elvira‐García
2023. Human self‐domestication and the evolution of prosody. Language and Linguistics Compass 17:3 DOI logo
CASTILLO, Eva, Mariia PRONINA, Iris HÜBSCHER & Pilar PRIETO
2023. Narrative Performance and Sociopragmatic Abilities in Preschool Children are Linked to Multimodal Imitation Skills. Journal of Child Language 50:1  pp. 52 ff. DOI logo
Hahn, Laura E., Titia Benders, Tineke M. Snijders & Paula Fikkert
2020. Six‐month‐old infants recognize phrases in song and speech. Infancy 25:5  pp. 699 ff. DOI logo
Hübscher, Iris & Pilar Prieto
2019. Gestural and Prosodic Development Act as Sister Systems and Jointly Pave the Way for Children’s Sociopragmatic Development. Frontiers in Psychology 10 DOI logo
Marimon, Mireia & Barbara Höhle
2022. Testing prosodic development with the Headturn Preference Procedure: A test‐retest reliability study. Infant and Child Development 31:6 DOI logo
Pronina, Mariia, Iris Hübscher, Ingrid Vilà-Giménez & Pilar Prieto
2021. Bridging the Gap Between Prosody and Pragmatics: The Acquisition of Pragmatic Prosody in the Preschool Years and Its Relation With Theory of Mind. Frontiers in Psychology 12 DOI logo
Pronina, Mariia, Pilar Prieto, Luca Bischetti & Valentina Bambini
2023. Expressive Pragmatics and Prosody in Young Preschoolers are More Closely Related to Structural Language than to Mentalizing. Language Learning and Development 19:3  pp. 323 ff. DOI logo
Sobhy, Ossama A., Rania M. Abdou, Sara Magdy Ibrahim & Nesrine H. Hamouda
2022. Effects of Prosody Rehabilitation on Acoustic Analysis of Prosodic Features in Hearing-Impaired Children: A Randomized Controlled Trial. Folia Phoniatrica et Logopaedica 74:1  pp. 29 ff. DOI logo
Zanchi, Paola, Sue Peppé & Laura Zampini
2022. Prosodic development from 4 to 10 years: Data from the Italian adaptation of the PEPS-C. Speech Communication 144  pp. 10 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 22 april 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.