Chapter published in:
The Development of Prosody in First Language Acquisition
Edited by Pilar Prieto and Núria Esteve-Gibert
[Trends in Language Acquisition Research 23] 2018
► pp. 79100
References

References

Aslin, R. N., Woodward, J. Z., LaMendola, N. P., & Bever, T. G.
(1996) Models of word segmentation in fluent maternal speech to infants. In J. L. Morgan, & K. Demuth (Eds.), Signal to syntax: Bootstrapping from speech to grammar in early acquisition (pp.117–134). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Bergelson, E., & Swingley, D.
(2012) At 6–9 months, human infants know the meanings of many common nouns. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(9), 3253–3258. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bernstein-Ratner, N.
(1996) From signal to syntax: But what is the nature of the signal? In J. L. Morgan, & K. Demuth (Eds.), Signal to syntax: Bootstrapping from speech to grammar in early acquisition (pp.135–150). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Bortfeld, H., Morgan, J. L., Golinkoff, R. M., & Rathbun, K.
(2005) Mommy and me: Familiar names help launch babies into speech-stream segmentation. Psychological Science, 16(4), 298–304. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bosch, L., Figueras, M., Teixidó, M., & Ramon-Casas, M.
(2013) Rapid gains in segmenting fluent speech when words match the rhythmic unit: Evidence from infants acquiring syllable-timed languages. Frontiers in Psychology, 4(106). https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00106.Google Scholar
Bosseler, A. N., Teinonen, T., Tervaniemi, M., & Huotilainen, M.
(2016) Infant-directed speech enhances statistical learning in newborn infants: An ERP study. PloS one, 11(9), e0162177.CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Brandt, A. K., Gebrian, M., & Slevc, R.
(2012) Music and early language acquisition. Frontiers in Psychology, 3(327). https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00327.Google Scholar
Brent, M. R., & Siskind, J. M.
(2001) The role of exposure to isolated words in early vocabulary development. Cognition, 81(2), B33–B44. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cheour-Luhtanen, M., Alho, K., Kujala, T., Sainio, K., Reinikainen, K., Renlund, M., Aaltonen, O., Eerola, O., & Näätänen, R.
(1995) Mismatch negativity indicates vowel discrimination in newborns. Hearing Research, 82(1), 53–58. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Conboy, B. T., Rivera-Gaxiola, M., Silva-Pereyra, J., & Kuhl, P. K.
(2008) Event-related potential studies of early language processing at the phoneme, word, and sentence levels. Early Language Development, 5, 23–64. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cristia, A.
(2013) Input to language: The phonetics and perception of infant-directed speech. Language and Linguistics Compass, 7(3), 157–170. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Curtin, S., Campbell, J., & Hufnagle, D.
(2012) Mapping novel labels to actions: How the rhythm of words guides infants’ learning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 112(2), 127–140. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Eggermont, J. J., & Moore, J. K.
(2012) Morphological and functional development of the auditory nervous system. In L. A. Werner, R. R. Fay, & A. N. Popper (Eds.), Human auditory development (pp.61–105). New York, NY: Springer. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fernald, A.
(1992) Human maternal vocalizations to infants as biologically relevant signals: An evolutionary perspective. In J. H. Berkow (Ed.), The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture (pp.391–428). London: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Fernald, A., & Mazzie, C.
(1991) Prosody and focus in speech to infants and adults. Developmental Psychology, 27(2), 209–221. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fernald, A., & Morikawa, H.
(1993) Common themes and cultural variations in Japanese and American mothers’ speech to infants. Child Development, 64(3), 637–656. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fernald, A., Taeschner, T., Dunn, J., Papousek, M., Boysson-Bardies, B., & Fukui, I.
(1989) A cross-language study of prosodic modifications in mothers’ and fathers’ speech to preverbal infants. Journal of Child Language, 16, 477–501. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Friedrich, M., & Friederici, A. D.
(2006) Early N400 development and later language acquisition. Psychophysiology, 43(1), 1–12. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2006.00381.x CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Flaugnacco, E., Lopez, L., Terribili, C., Montico, M., Zoia, S., & Schön, D.
(2015) Music training increases phonological awareness and reading skills in developmental dyslexia: A randomized control trial. PloS one, 10(9), e0138715. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
François, C., Grau-Sánchez, J., Duarte, E., & Rodriguez-Fornells, A.
(2015) Musical training as an alternative and effective method for neuro-education and neuro-rehabilitation. Frontiers in Psychology, 6(475). https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00475.Google Scholar
François, C., Teixidó, M., Takerkart, S., Agut, T., Bosch, L., & Rodriguez-Fornells, A.
(2017) Enhanced Neonatal Brain Responses To Sung Streams Predict Vocabulary Outcomes By Age 18 Months. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 12451. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-12798-2 CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Friederici, A. D.
(2005) Neurophysiological markers of early language acquisition: from syllables to sentences. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(10), 481–488. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Friederici, A. D., & Männel, C.
(2013) Neural correlates of the development of speech perception and comprehension. In K. N. Ochsner, & S. M. Kosslyn (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of cognitive neuroscience (Vol. 1, pp.171–192). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Friedrich, M., & Friederici, A. D.
(2004) N400-like semantic incongruity effect in 19-month-olds: Processing known words in picture contexts. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(8), 1465–1477. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2005a) Phonotactic knowledge and lexical-semantic processing in one-year-olds: Brain responses to words and nonsense words in picture contexts. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(11), 1785–1802. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2005b) Lexical priming and semantic integration reflected in the event-related potential of 14-month-olds. Neuroreport, 16(6), 653–656. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2008) Neurophysiological correlates of online word learning in 14-month-old infants. Neuroreport, 19(18), 1757–1761. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2011) Word learning in 6-month-olds: Fast encoding–weak retention. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(11), 3228–3240. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2015) The origins of word learning: Brain responses of three-month-olds indicate their rapid association of objects and words. Developmental Science, 20(2), e12357.Google Scholar
Gervain, J., & Werker, J. F.
(2013) Prosody cues word order in 7-month-old bilingual infants. Nature Communications, 4, 1490–1496. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gogate, L. J., Bolzani, L. H., & Betancourt, E. A.
(2006) Attention to maternal multimodal naming by 6- to 8-month-old infants and learning of word-object relations. Infancy, 9(3), 259–288. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gogate, L. J., Prince, C. G., & Matatyaho, D. J.
(2009) Two-month-old infants’ sensitivity to changes in arbitrary syllable–object pairings: The role of temporal synchrony. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35(2), 508.Google Scholar
Goyet, L., de Schonen, S., & Nazzi, T.
(2010) Words and syllables in fluent speech segmentation by French-learning infants: An ERP study. Brain Research, 1332, 75–89. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Goyet, L., Millotte, S., Christophe, A., & Nazzi, T.
(2016) Processing continuous speech in Infancy: From major prosodic units to isolated word forms. In W. Snyder, & J. Pater (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of developmental linguistics, (Vol. 1, pp.133–156). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Graf-Estes, K., & Hurley, K.
(2013) Infant-directed prosody helps infants map sounds to meanings. Infancy, 18(5), 797–824. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Graf-Estes, K., & Bowen, S.
(2013) Learning about sounds contributes to learning about words: Effects of prosody and phonotactics on infant word learning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 114(3), 405–417. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Grassmann, S., & Tomasello, M.
(2007) Two-year-olds use primary sentence accent to learn new words. Journal of Child Language, 34(03), 677–687. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2010) Prosodic stress on a word directs 24-month-olds’ attention to a contextually new referent. Journal of Pragmatics, 42(11), 3098–3105. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Heffner, C. C., & Slevc, L. R.
(2015) Prosodic structure as a parallel to musical structure. Frontiers in Psychology, 6(1962). http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01962.Google Scholar
Höhle, B., & Weissenborn, J.
(2003) German-learning infants’ ability to detect unstressed closed-class elements in continuous speech. Developmental Science, 6(2), 122–127. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Holcomb, P. J.
(1993) Semantic priming and stimulus degradation: Implications for the role of the N400 in language processing. Psychophysiology, 30(1), 47–61. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Houston, D. M., Jusczyk, P. W., Kuijpers, C., Coolen, R., & Cutler, A.
(2000) Cross-language word segmentation by 9-month-olds. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 7(3), 504–509. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Junge, C., Kooijman, V., Hagoort, P., & Cutler, A.
(2012) Rapid recognition at 10 months as a predictor of language development. Developmental Science, 15(4), 463–473. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Jusczyk, P. W.
(1999) How infants begin to extract words from speech. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3(9), 323–328. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Jusczyk, P. W., & Aslin, R. N.
(1995) Infants’ detection of the sound patterns of words in fluent speech. Cognitive Psychology, 29(1), 1–23. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Jusczyk, P. W., Houston, D. M., & Newsome, M.
(1999) The beginnings of word segmentation in English-learning infants. Cognitive Psychology, 39(3), 159–207. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kitamura, C., & Burnham, D.
(2003) Pitch and communicative intent in mother’s speech: Adjustments for age and sex in the first year. Infancy, 4(1), 85–110. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kooijman, V., Hagoort, P., & Cutler, A.
(2005) Electrophysiological evidence for prelinguistic infants’ word recognition in continuous speech. Cognitive Brain Research, 24, 109–116. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2009) Prosodic structure in early word segmentation: ERP evidence from Dutch ten-month-olds. Infancy, 14(6), 591–612. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kooijman, V., Junge, C., Johnson, E. K., Hagoort, P., & Cutler, A.
(2013) Predictive brain signals of linguistic development. Frontiers in Psychology, 4(25). https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00025.Google Scholar
Kuhl, P. K.
(2010) Brain mechanisms in early language acquisition. Neuron, 67(5), 713–727. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kuijpers, C., Coolen, R., Houston, D., & Cutler, A.
(1998) Using the head-turning technique to explore cross-linguistic performance differences. In C. Rovee-Collier, L. Lipsitt, & H. Hayne (Eds.), Advances in infancy research (Vol. 12, pp.205–220). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Kutas, M., van Petten, C., & Kluender, R.
(2006) Psycholinguistics electrified II (1994–2005). In M. Traxler, & M. A. Gernsbacher (Eds.), Handbook of psycholinguistics (Vol. 1, pp.83–143). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lebedeva, G. C., & Kuhl, P. K.
(2010) Sing that tune: Infants’ perception of melody and lyrics and the facilitation of phonetic recognition in songs. Infant Behavior and Development, 33(4), 419–430. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ma, W., Golinkoff, R. M., Houston, D. M., & Hirsh-Pasek, K.
(2011) Word learning in infant- and adult-directed speech. Language Learning and Development, 7(3), 185–201. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Männel, C. & Friederici, A. D.
(2008) Event-related brain potentials as a window to children’s language processing: From syllables to sentences. In I. A. Sekerina, E. M. Fernández, H. Clahsen (Eds.), Developmental psycholinguistics: On-line methods in children’s language processing (pp.29–72). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2010) Prosody is the key: ERP studies on word segmentation in 6- and 12-month-old children. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, Supplement, 261.Google Scholar
Männel, C., & Friederici, A. D.
(2013) Accentuate or repeat? Brain signatures of developmental periods in infant word recognition. Cortex, 49(10), 2788–2798. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Männel, C., Teixidó, M., Bosch, L., Friederici, A. D. & Friedrich, M.
(November, 2017) Sentence prosody cues object category learning at 6 months. Paper presented at the Ninth Annual Meeting of the Society for the Neurobiology of Language (SNL), Baltimore, USA.
Männel, C.
(2008) The method of event-related brain potentials in the study of cognitive processes. A tutorial. In A. D. Friederici, & G. Thierry (Eds.), Early language development: Bridging brain and behaviour, (pp.1–22). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Matatyaho, D. J., & Gogate, L. J.
(2008) Type of maternal object motion during synchronous naming predicts preverbal infants’ learning of word–object relations. Infancy, 13(2), 172–184. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Mills, D. L., Coffey-Corina, S. A., & Neville, H. J.
(1993) Language acquisition and cerebral specialization in 20-month-old infants. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 5(3), 317–334. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(1997) Language comprehension and cerebral specialization from 13 to 20 months. Developmental Neuropsychology, 13(3), 397–445. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Moore, J. K., & Guan, Y. L.
(2001) Cytoarchitectural and axonal maturation in human auditory cortex. Journal of the Association for Research in Otolaryngology, 2(4), 297–311. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Moore, J. K., & Linthicum Jr, F. H.
(2007) The human auditory system: A timeline of development. International Journal of Audiology, 46(9), 460–478. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Mueller, J. L., Friederici, A. D., & Männel, C.
(2012) Auditory perception at the root of language learning. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(39), 15953–15958. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Nazzi, T., & Bertoncini, J.
(2003) Before and after the vocabulary spurt: Two modes of word acquisition? Developmental Science, 6(2), 136–142. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Nazzi, T., Iakimova, G., Bertoncini, J., Frédonie, S., & Alcantara, C.
(2006) Early segmentation of fluent speech by infants acquiring French: Emerging evidence for crosslinguistic differences. Journal of Memory and Language, 54(3), 283–299. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Newman, R. S., & Hussain, I.
(2006) Changes in preference for infant-directed speech in low and moderate noise by 4.5- to 13-month-olds. Infancy, 10(1), 61–76. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Newman, R., Ratner, N. B., Jusczyk, A. M., Jusczyk, P. W., & Dow, K. A.
(2006) Infants’ early ability to segment the conversational speech signal predicts later language development: A retrospective analysis. Developmental Psychology, 42(4), 643–655. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Papousek, M., Papousek, H., & Bornstein, M.
(1985) The naturalistic vocal environment of young infants: On the significance of homogeneity and variability in parental speech. In T. M. Field, & N. A. Fox (Eds.), Social perception in infants (pp.269–297). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Patel, A. D.
(2008) Science & music: Talk of the tone. Nature, 453(7196), 726–727. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Polka, L., & Sundara, M.
(2012) Word segmentation in monolingual infants acquiring Canadian English and Canadian French: Native language, cross-dialect, and cross-language comparisons. Infancy, 17(2), 198–232. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Schön, D., Boyer, M., Moreno, S., Besson, M., Peretz, I., & Kolinsky, R.
(2008) Songs as an aid for language acquisition. Cognition, 106(2), 975–983. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Shukla, M., White, K. S., & Aslin, R. N.
(2011) Prosody guides the rapid mapping of auditory word forms onto visual objects in 6-mo-old infants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(15), 6038–6043. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Singh, L., Steven Reznick, J., & Xuehua, L.
(2012) Infant word segmentation and childhood vocabulary development: A longitudinal analysis. Developmental Science, 15(4), 482–495. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Soderstrom, M.
(2007) Beyond babytalk: Re-evaluating the nature and content of speech input to preverbal infants. Developmental Review, 27, 501–532. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Stefanics, G., Háden, G. P., Sziller, I., Balázs, L., Beke, A., & Winkler, I.
(2009) Newborn infants process pitch intervals. Clinical Neurophysiology, 120(2), 304–308. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Stern, D. N., Spieker, S., Barnett, R. K., & MacKain, K.
(1983) The prosody of maternal speech: Infant age and context related changes. Journal of Child Language, 10(01), 1–15. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Teixidó, M., & Bosch, L.
(2014) Early segmentation and word mapping in a natural language: Evidence from 4-, 6- and 9-months-olds. Paper presented at the XIX Biennial International Conference on Infant Studies (ICIS), Berlin, Germany.Google Scholar
Thierry, G., Vihman, M., & Roberts, M.
(2003) Familiar words capture the attention of 11-month-olds in less than 250 ms. Neuroreport, 14(18), 2307–2310. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Thiessen, E. D., & Saffran, J. R.
(2009) How the melody facilitates the message and vice versa in infant learning and memory. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1169(1), 225–233. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Thiessen, E. D., Hill, E. A., & Saffran, J. R.
(2005) Infant-directed speech facilitates word segmentation. Infancy, 7(1), 53–71. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Thorson, J. C. & Morgan, J. L.
(2015) Acoustic correlates of information structure in child and adult speech. In E. Grillo & K. Jepson (Eds.), Proceedings of the 39th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp.411–423). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Unyk, A. M., Trehub, S. E., Trainor, L. J., & Schellenberg, E. G.
(1992) Lullabies and simplicity: A cross-cultural perspective. Psychology of Music, 20(1), 15–28. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
von Koss Torkildsen, J., Hansen, H. F., Svangstu, J. M., Smith, L., Simonsen, H. G., Moen, I., & Lindgren, M.
(2009) Brain dynamics of word familiarization in 20-month-olds: Effects of productive vocabulary size. Brain and Language, 108(2), 73–88. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Zhao, T. C., & Kuhl, P. K.
(2016) Musical intervention enhances infants’ neural processing of temporal structure in music and speech. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113(19), 5212–5217.CrossrefGoogle Scholar