Chapter published in:
The Development of Prosody in First Language Acquisition
Edited by Pilar Prieto and Núria Esteve-Gibert
[Trends in Language Acquisition Research 23] 2018
► pp. 185206
References

References

Boysson-Bardies, B.de, & Vihman, M. M.
(1991) Adaptation to language: Evidence from babbling and first words in four languages. Language, 67, 297–319. Crossref logo
Brent, M. R., & Siskind, J. M.
(2001) The role of exposure to isolated words in early vocabulary development. Cognition, 81, B33–B44. Crossref logo
Bybee, J.
(2001) Phonology and language use. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Crossref logo
(2010) Language, usage and cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Crossref logo
Cutler, A., & Carter, D. M.
(1987) The predominance of strong initial syllables in the English vocabulary. Computer Speech and Language, 2, 133–142. Crossref logo
Davis, B. L., & MacNeilage, P. F.
(2000) An embodiment perspective on the acquisition of speech perception. Phonetica, 57, 229–241. Crossref logo
DePaolis, R. A., Keren-Portnoy, T., & Vihman, M. M.
(2016) Making sense of infant familiarity and novelty responses to words at lexical onset. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 715. Crossref logo
DePaolis, R., Vihman, M. M., & Keren-Portnoy, T.
(2011) Do production patterns influence the processing of speech in prelinguistic infants? Infant Behavior and Development, 34, 590–601. Crossref logo
DePaolis, R. A., Vihman, M. M., & Nakai, S.
(2013) The influence of babbling patterns on the processing of speech. Infant Behavior and Development, 36, 642–649. Crossref logo
Ferguson, C. A., & Farwell, C. B.
(1975) Words and sounds in early language acquisition. Language, 51, 419–439. Crossref logo
Fernald, A.
(1991) Prosody in speech to children: Prelinguistic and linguistic functions. Annals of Child Development, 8, 43–80.
Fikkert, P.
(1994) On the acquisition of prosodic structure. Amsterdam: Holland Institute of Generative Linguistics.
Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D.
(1989) Evaluation of the role of phonological STM in the development of vocabulary in children: A longitudinal study. Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 200–213. Crossref logo
Goldsmith, J. A.
(1990) Autosegmental and metrical phonology. Oxford: Blackwell.
Grabe, E., & Low, E. L.
(2002) Durational variability in speech and the rhythm class hypothesis. In C. Gussenhoven, & N. Warner (Eds.), Papers in laboratory phonology VII (pp.515–546). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Crossref logo
Hall, K. C., Hume, E., Jaeger, T. F., & Wedel, A.
(2016) The message shapes phonology.Paper presented at the symposium on The role of predictability in shaping human language sound patterns, Sydney, Australia
Icht, M., & Mama, Y.
(2015) The production effect in memory: A prominent mnemonic in children. Journal of Child Language, 42, 1102–1124. Crossref logo
Jusczyk, P. W.
(1993) From general to language-specific capacities: The WRAPSA model of how speech perception develops. Journal of Phonetics, 21, 3–28.
(1997) The discovery of spoken language. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
Kehoe, M.
(2013) The development of prosody and prosodic structure. New York, NY: Nova Science Publishers.
Kehoe, M., & Stoel-Gammon, C.
(2001) Development of syllable structure in English-speaking children with particular reference to rhymes. Journal of Child Language, 28, 393–432. Crossref logo
Kehoe, M., & Lleó, C.
(2003) The acquisition of nuclei: A longitudinal analysis of phonological vowel length in three German-speaking children. Journal of Child Language, 30, 527–556. Crossref logo
Keren-Portnoy, T., Majorano, M., & Vihman, M. M.
(2009) From phonetics to phonology: The emergence of first words in Italian. Journal of Child Language, 36, 235–267. Crossref logo
Keren-Portnoy, T., & Segal, O.
(2016) Phonological development in Israeli-Hebrew-learning infants and toddlers: Perception and perception. In R. A. Berman (Ed.), Acquisition and development of Hebrew (pp.69–94). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Crossref logo
Keren-Portnoy, T., Vihman, M. M., DePaolis, R., Whitaker, C., & Williams, N. A.
(2010) The role of vocal practice in constructing phonological working memory. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 53, 1280–1293. Crossref logo
Khattab, G., & Al-Tamimi, J.
(2013) Early phonological patterns in Lebanese Arabic. In M. M. Vihman, & T. Keren-Portnoy (Eds.), The emergence of phonology: Whole word approaches, cross-linguistic evidence (pp.374–414). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Crossref logo
Kuhl, P. K.
(1987) Perception of speech and sound in early infancy. In P. Salapatek, & L. Cohen (Eds.), Handbook of infant perception (Vol. 2, pp.275–381). New York, NY: Academic Press.
Kunnari, S.
(2000) Characteristics of early lexical and phonological development in children acquiring Finnish. Unpublished PhD dissertation. University of Oulu.
Macken, M. A.
(1979) Developmental reorganization of phonology: A hierarchy of basic units of acquisition. Lingua, 49, 11–49. Crossref logo
MacLeod, C., Gopie, N., Hourihan, K. L., Neary, K. R., & Ozubko, K. D.
(2010) The production effect: Delineation of a phenomenon. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36, 671–685.
Majorano, M., Vihman, M. M., & DePaolis, R. A.
(2014) The relationship between infants’ production experience and their processing of speech. Language Learning and Development, 10, 179–204. Crossref logo
Mehler, J., Dupoux, E., Nazzi, T., & Dehaene-Lambertz, G.
(1996) Coping with linguistic diversity: The infant’s viewpoint. In J. Morgan, & K. Demuth (Eds.), From signal to syntax: Bootstrapping from speech to grammar in early acquisition (pp.101–116). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Mehler, J., Jusczyk, P. Lambertz, G., Halsted, N., Bertoncini, J., & Amiel-Tison, C.
(1988) A precursor of language acquisition in young infants. Cognition, 29, 143–178. Crossref logo
Menn, L., & Vihman, M. M.
(2011) Features in child phonology: Inherent, emergent, or artefacts of analysis? In N. Clements, & R. Ridouane (Eds.), Where do phonological features come from? (pp.261–301). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Crossref logo
Munson, B., Edwards, J., & Beckman, M. E.
(2012) Phonological representation in language acquisition: Climbing the ladder of abstraction. In A. C. Cohn, C. Fougeron, & M. K. Huffman (Eds.), Oxford handbook of laboratory phonology (pp.288–309). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Nespor, M., & Vogel, I.
(1986) Prosodic phonology. Dordrecht: Foris.
Oller, D. K.
(1980) The emergence of the sounds of speech in infancy. In G. Yeni-Komshian, J. F. Kavanagh, & C. A. Ferguson (Eds.), Child phonology, Vol. I: Production (pp.93–112). New York, NY: Academic Press.Crossref logo
Oller, D. K., & Eilers, R. E.
(1988) The role of audition in infant babbling. Child Development, 59, 441–449. Crossref logo
Payne, E., Post, B., Astruc, L., Prieto, P., & Vanrell, M. M.
(2012) Measuring child rhythm. Language and Speech, 55, 203–229. Crossref logo
Pierrehumbert, J.
(2003) Phonetic diversity, statistical learning, and acquisition of phonology. Language and Speech, 46, 115–154. Crossref logo
Priestly, T. M. S.
(1977) One idiosyncratic strategy in the aquisition of phonology. Journal of Child Language, 4, 45–66. Crossref logo
Ramus, F., Nespor, M., & Mehler, J.
(1999) Correlates of linguistic rhythm in the speech signal. Cognition, 73, 265–292. Crossref logo
Savinainen-Makkonen, T.
(2000a) Learning long words: A typological perspective. Language and Speech, 43, 205–225. Crossref logo
(2000b) Word initial consonant omissions: A developmental process in children learning Finnish. First Language, 20, 161–185. Crossref logo
Selkirk, E.
(1978) On prosodic structure and its relation to syntactic structure. In T. Fretheim (Ed.), Nordic prosody II (pp.111–140). Trondheim: Tapir.
(1980) The role of prosodic categories in English word stress. Linguistic Inquiry, 11, 563–605.
(1996) The prosodic structure of function words. In J. Morgan, & K. Demuth (Eds.), From signal to syntax: Bootstrapping from speech to grammar in early acquisition (pp.187–213). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Stoel-Gammon, C., & Cooper, J. A.
(1984) Patterns of early lexical and phonological development. Journal of Child Language, 11, 247–271. Crossref logo
Veneziano, E., & Sinclair, H.
(2000) The changing status of ‘filler syllables’ on the way to grammatical morphemes. Journal of Child Language, 27, 461–500. Crossref logo
Vihman, M. M.
(1993) Variable paths to early word production. Journal of Phonetics, 21, 61–82.
(1996) Phonological development: The origins of language in the child. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.
(2010) Phonological templates in early words: A cross-linguistic study. In C. Fougeron, B. Kühnert, M. D’Imperio, & N. Vallée (Eds.), Laboratory phonology 10 (pp.261–284). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
(2014) Phonological development: The first two years (2nd ed.). Malden, MA: Wiley–Blackwell.
(2016) Prosodic structures and templates in bilingual phonological development. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19, 69–88. Crossref logo
(in press). First word learning. In S-A. Rueschmeyer, & G. Gaskell Eds. Oxford handbook of psycholinguistics. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Vihman, M. M., & Croft, W.
(2007) Phonological development: Toward a ‘radical’ templatic phonology. Linguistics, 45, 683–725. Crossref logo
Vihman, M. M., Kay, E., Boysson-Bardies, B., de, Durand, C., & Sundberg, U.
(1994) External sources of individual differences? A cross-linguistic analysis of the phonetics of mothers’ speech to one-year-old children. Developmental Psychology, 30, 652–663. Crossref logo
Vihman, M. M., & Keren-Portnoy, T.
(Eds.) (2013) The emergence of phonology: Whole word approaches, cross-linguistic evidence. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Crossref logo
Vihman, M. M., Keren-Portnoy, T., Khattab, G., & Wauquier, S.
(2011, July). Psychological significance and typology of templates in early word production. Paper presented at International Association for the Study of Child Language, Montreal, Canada.
Vihman, M. M., & Kunnari, S.
(2006) The sources of phonological knowledge: A cross-linguistic perspective. Recherches Linguistiques de Vincennes, 35, 133–164. Crossref logo
Vihman, M. M., Macken, M. A., Miller, R., Simmons, H., & Miller, J.
(1985) From babbling to speech: A reassessment of the continuity issue. Language, 61, 397–445. Crossref logo
Vihman, M. M., & Majorano, M.
(2017) The role of geminates in infants’ early words and word-form recognition. Journal of Child Language, 44, 158–184.Crossref logo
Vihman, M. M., Nakai, S., & DePaolis, R. A.
(2006) Getting the rhythm right: A cross-linguistic study of segmental duration in babbling and first words. In L. Goldstein, D. Whalen, & C. Best (Eds.), Laboratory Phonology 8 (pp.341–366). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
Vihman, M. M., Nakai, S., DePaolis, R. A., & Hallé, P.
(2004) The role of accentual pattern in early lexical representation. Journal of Memory and Language, 50, 336–353. Crossref logo
Vihman, M. M., & Velleman, S.
(1989) Phonological reorganization: A case study. Language and Speech, 32, 149–170.Crossref logo
Vihman, M. M., & Velleman, S. L.
(2000) Phonetics and the origins of phonology. In N. Burton-Roberts, P. Carr & G. Docherty (Eds.), Phonological knowledge: Its nature and status (pp.305–339). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Vihman, M. M., Velleman, S. L. & McCune, L.
(1994) How abstract is child phonology? Towards an integration of linguistic and psychological approaches. In M. Yavas (Ed.), First and second language phonology (pp.9–44). San Diego, CA: Singular.
Wauquier, S., & Yamaguchi, N.
(2013) Templates in French. In M. M. Vihman & T. Keren-Portnoy (Eds.), The emergence of phonology: Whole-word approaches, cross-linguistic evidence (pp.317–342). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Crossref logo
Zamuner, T. S., Morin-Lessard, E., Strahm, S., & Page, M. P. A.
(2016) Spoken word recognition of novel words, either produced or only heard during learning. Journal of Memory and Language, 89, 55–67.doi:Crossref logo