Chapter 7
Links between language and cognitive development of deaf children
This chapter weaves together work on early interaction between parents and children who are deaf or hard of hearing (DHH) with research on social-emotional development (Theory of Mind) and wider cognitive abilities (Executive Functions). We describe in detail why language input in sign or spoken language (or both together) facilitates the development of communication, language and cognitive skills using what has been termed the Language Scaffolding Hypothesis. The chapter concludes with a discussion of what research is required next to understand how the current language learning experiences of DHH children – the majority who will have a cochlear implant and hearing parents – can promote both language and cognitive development.
Article outline
- 1.Introduction
- 2.What is the impact of deafness on language development?
- 3.Language for cognitive development: Theory of Mind and Executive Functions
- 4.General conclusions
-
References
References (58)
References
Astington, J. W., & Baird, J. (2005). Why language matters for Theory of Mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Best, J. R., & Miller, P. H. (2010). A developmental perspective on executive function. Child Development 81(6), 1641–1660.
Bohlmann, N. L., Maier, M. F., & Palacios, N. (2015). Bidirectionality in self-regulation and expressive vocabulary: Comparisons between monolingual and dual language learners in preschool. Child Development 86(4), 109.
Botting, N., Jones, A., Marshall, C., Denmark, T., Atkinson, J., & Morgan, G. (2017). Nonverbal executive function is mediated by language: A study of deaf and hearing children. Child development 88(5), 1689–1700.
Bruner, J. S. (1983). Child’s talk: Learning to use language. New York, NY: Norton.
Conway, C. M., Pisoni, D. B., & Kronenberger, W. G. (2009). The importance of sound for cognitive sequencing abilities the auditory scaffolding hypothesis. Current Directions in Psychological Science 18(5), 275–279.
Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology 64, 135–168.
Elliott, R. (2003). Executive functions and their disorders imaging in clinical neuroscience. British Medical Bulletin 65, 49–59.
Fagan, M. K., Bergeson, T. R., & Morris, K. J. (2014). Synchrony, complexity and directiveness in mothers’ interactions with infants pre- and post-cochlear implantation. Infant Behavior & Development 37(3), 249–257.
Figueras, B., Edwards, L., & Langdon, D. (2008). Executive function and language in deaf children. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 13(3), 362–377.
Fuhs, M. W., & Day, J. D. (2011). Verbal ability and executive functioning development in preschoolers at head start. Developmental Psychology 47(2), 404–416.
Funahashi, S. (2001). Neuronal mechanisms of executive control by the prefrontal cortex. Neuroscience Research 39, 147–165.
Geers, A. E., Nicholas, J. G., Tobey, E., & Davidson, L. (2016). Persistent language delay versus late language emergence in children with early cochlear implantation. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research 59(1), 155–170.
Hall, M., Eigsti, I., Bortfeld, H., & Lillo-Martin, D. (2017). Auditory deprivation does not impair executive function, but language deprivation might: Evidence from a parent-report measure in deaf native signing children. The Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 22, 9–21.
Hughes, C., White, N., & Ensor, R. (2014). How does talk about thoughts, desires, and feelings foster children’s socio-cognitive development? Mediators, moderators and implications for intervention. In K. H. Lagattuta (Ed), Children and emotion. New insights into developmental affective sciences (pp.95–105). Basel: Karger.
Jones, A., Marshall, C., Botting, N., Atkinson, J., Toscana, E., Denmark, T., Herman, R., & Morgan, G. (2016). Narrative skills in deaf children who use spoken English: dissociations between macro and microstructural devices. Research in Developmental Disabilities 59, 268–282.
Jones, A., Atkinson, J., Marshall, C., Botting, N., St Clair, M. C., Morgan, G. (2019). Expressive vocabulary predicts non-verbal executive function: A 2-year longitudinal study of deaf and hearing children. Child Development.
Kuhn, L. J., Willoughby, M. T., Wilbourn, M. P., Vernon-Feagans, L., & Blair, C. B. (2014). Early communicative gestures prospectively predict language development and executive function in early childhood. Child Development 85, 1898–1914.
Kyle, J. G., Woll, B., & Ackerman, J. A. (1989). Gesture to sign and speech. Bristol: Centre for Deaf Studies.
Lederberg, A., & Mobley, C. (1990). The effect of hearing impairment on the quality of attachment and mother–toddler interaction. Child Development 61, 1596–1604.
Levine, D., Strother-Garcia, K., Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Golinkoff, R. M. (2016). Language development in the first year of life: What deaf children might be missing before cochlear implantation. Otology & Neurotology 37, 56–62.
Lidstone, J., Meins, E., & Fernyhough, C. (2012). Verbal mediation of cognition in children with specific language impairment. Development and Psychopathology 24(2), 651–660.
Lieberman, A. M., Hatrak, M., & Mayberry, R. (2014). Learning to Look for Language: Development of joint attention in young deaf children. Language Learning and Development 10, 19–35.
Loots, G., Devisé, I., & Jacquet, W. (2005). The impact of visual communication on the intersubjective development of early parent–child interaction with 18- to 24-month-old deaf toddlers. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 10, 357–375.
Lowe, J. R., MacLean, P. C., Duncan, A. F., Aragón, C., Schrader, R. M., Caprihan, A., & Phillips, J. P. (2012). Association of maternal interaction with emotional regulation in 4 and 9 month infants during the still face paradigm. Infant Behavior & Development 35(2), 295–302.
Lu, J., Jones, A. & Morgan, G. (2016). The impact of input quality on early sign development in native and non-native language learners. Journal of Child Language 43, 537–552.
Marshall C., Jones A., Denmark T., Mason K., Atkinson J., Botting N., & Morgan G. (2015). Deaf children’s non-verbal working memory is impacted by their language experience. Front. Psychol 6, 527.
Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., Arnott, B., Leekam, S., & de Rosnay, M. (2013). Mind- mindedness and Theory of Mind: Mediating roles of language and perspectival symbolic play. Child Development 84(5): 1777–1790.
Mitchell, R., & Karchmer, M. (2004). When parents are deaf versus hard of hearing: Patterns of sign use and school placement of deaf and hard-of-hearing children. The Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 9, 133–152.
Morgan, G., Meristo, M., & Hjelmquist, E. (2014). Conversational experience, language and the development of Theory of Mind. In V. Slaughter (Ed.), Environmental influences on Theory of Mind development: Festschrift for Candi Peterson. Hove: Psychology Press.
Morgan, G., Meristo, M. Mann, W., Hjelmquist, E., Surian, L., & Siegal, M. (2014). Mental state language and quality of conversational experience in deaf and hearing children. Cognitive Development 29, 41–49.
Netten, A. P., Rieffe, C., Theunissen, S., Soede, W., Dirks, E., & Briaire, J. (2015). Low empathy in deaf and hard of hearing (pre) adolescents compared to normal hearing controls. PLoS ONE 10(4): e0124102.
Nowakowski, M. E., Tasker, S. L., & Schmidt L. A. (2009). Establishment of joint attention in dyads involving hearing mothers of deaf and hearing children, and its relation to adaptive social behavior. American Annals of the Deaf 154(1),15–29.
Pierce, L., Genesee, F., Delcenserie, A., & Morgan, G. (2017). Variations in phonological working memory: Linking early language experiences and language learning outcomes. Applied Psycholinguistics 38(6), 1265–1300.
Peterson, C. C. (2004). Theory of Mind development in oral deaf children with cochlear implants or conventional hearing aids. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 45, 1096–1106.
Peterson, C. C., Wellman, H. M., & Slaughter, V. (2012). The mind behind the message: Advancing theory of mind scales for typically developing children, and those with deafness, autism, or Asperger Syndrome. Child Development 83, 469–485.
Remine, M. D., Care, E., & Brown, P. M. (2008). Language ability and verbal and nonverbal executive functioning in deaf students communicating in spoken English. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 13, 531–545.
Schick, B., Marschark, M., & Spencer, P. (Eds.). (2005). Advances in the sign language development of deaf and hard-of-hearing children. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Schick, B., de Villiers, P., de Villiers, J., & Hoffmeister, R. (2007). Language and theory of mind: A study of deaf children. Child Development 78, 376–396.
Siegal, M., & Surian, L. (2011). Access to language and cognitive development. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Spencer, P., Bodner-Johnson, B., & Gutfreund, M. (1992). Interacting with infants with a hearing loss: What can we learn from mothers who are deaf? Journal of Early Intervention 16, 64–78.
Spencer, P., & Meadow-Orlans, K. (1996). Play, language, and maternal responsiveness: A longitudinal study of deaf and hearing infants. Child Development 67, 3176–3191.
Stern, D. N., & Gibbon, J. (1979). Temporal expectancies of social behaviors in mother–infant play. In A. W. Siegman & S. Feldstein (Eds.), Of speech and time. Temporal speech patterns in interpersonal contexts. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Swisher, M. V. (1992). The role of parents in developing visual turn-taking in their young deaf children. American Annals of the Deaf 137, 92–100. .
Tomasello, M (1988) The role of joint attentional processes in early language development. Language Sciences 10, 69–88.
Tomasello, M. (2008). Origins of human communication. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., & Moll, H. (2005). Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 28(5), 675–691.
Trevarthen, C., & Hubley, P. (1978). Secondary intersubjectivity: Confidence, confiding and acts of meaning in the first year. In A. Lock (Ed.), Action, gesture and symbol: The emergence of language (pp.183–229). New York, NY: Academic Press.
Vaccari, S. & Marschark, M. (1997). Communication between parents and deaf children: Implications for social-emotional development. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 38, 793–801.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1962). The problem and the approach. In Thought and language (pp.1–11). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
Wedell-Monning, J., & Lumley, J. M. (1980). Child deafness and mother–child interaction. Child Development 51, 766–774.
Weiland, C., Barata, M., & Yoshikawa, H. (2014). The co-occurring development of executive function skills and receptive vocabulary in preschool-aged children: A look at the direction of the developmental pathways. Infant and Child Development 23(1), 4–21.
Wellman, H. M., & Liu, D. (2004). Scaling of theory of mind tasks. Child Development 75, 523–541.
Woll, B. (2018). The consequences of very late exposure to BSL as an L1. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1–2.
Woolfe, T., Want, S. C., & Siegal, M. (2002). Signposts to development: Theory of Mind in deaf children. Child Development 73, 768–778.
Zelazo, P. D., & Frye, D. (1998). Cognitive complexity and control, II: The development of executive function in childhood. Current Directions in Psychological Science 7(4), 121–126. [URL].
Cited by (1)
Cited by one other publication
Figueroa, Mario, Nicola Botting & Gary Morgan
2024.
Language and executive function relationships in the real world: insights from deafness.
Language and Cognition ► pp. 1 ff.
This list is based on CrossRef data as of 29 july 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers.
Any errors therein should be reported to them.