Chapter published in:
Current Perspectives on Child Language Acquisition: How children use their environment to learn
Edited by Caroline F. Rowland, Anna L. Theakston, Ben Ambridge and Katherine E. Twomey
[Trends in Language Acquisition Research 27] 2020
► pp. 173187
References
Adamson, L., & Bakeman, R.
(1985) Affect and attention: Infants observed with mothers and peers. Child Development, 56, 582–593. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Austin, J. L.
(1962) How to do things with words. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bakeman, R., & Adamson, L.
(1984) Coordinating attention to people and objects in mother–infant and peer–infant interaction. Child Development, 55, 1278–1289. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bates, E., Camaioni, L., & Volterra, V.
(1975) The acquisition of performatives prior to speech. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 21, 205–226.Google Scholar
Boundy, L., Cameron-Faulkner, T., & Theakston, A.
(2019) Intention or attention before pointing: Do infants’ early hold out gestures reflect evidence of a declarative motive? Infancy 24, 228–248. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Brinck, I.
(2004) The pragmatics of imperative and declarative pointing. Cognitive Science Quarterly, 3, 429–446.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. S.
(1975) The ontogenesis of speech acts. Journal of Child Language, 2, 1–19. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(1983) Child’s Talk: Learning to use language. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Butterworth, G.
(2003) Pointing is the royal road to language for babies. In S. Kita (Ed.), Pointing: Where language, culture, and cognition meet. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Cameron-Faulkner, T., Theakston, A., Lieven, E. V., & Tomasello, M.
(2015) The relationship between infant hold out and gives, and pointing. Infancy, 20, 576–586. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Carpendale, J.
(2016) Communication as the coordination of activity: The implications of philosophical preconceptions for theories of the development of communication. In A. Dick & U. Müller (Eds.), Advancing developmental science: Philosophy, theory, and method. New York, NY: Taylor & Francis.Google Scholar
Carpendale, J., & Lewis, C.
(2004) Constructing an understanding of mind: The development of children’s social understanding within social interaction. Behavioural and Brian Sciences, 27, 79–96.Google Scholar
Carpenter, M., Nagell, K., Tomasello, M., Butterworth, G., & Moore, C.
(1998) Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 63, 1–174. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Colonnesi, C., Jan, G., Stams, J., Koster, I., & Noom, M.
(2010) The relation between pointing and language development: A meta-analysis. Developmental Review, 30, 352–366. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
de Barbaro, K., Johnson, C. M., Forster, D., & Deak, G. O.
(2016) Sensorimotor decoupling contributes to triadic attention: A longitudinal investigation of mother–infant–object interactions. Child Development, 87(2), 494–512. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
de Barbaro, K., Johnson, C., Forster, D., & Deak, G.
(2013) Methodological considerations for investigating the microdynamics of social interaction development. IEEE Transactions on Autonomous Mental Development, 5, 258–270. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dore, J.
(1975) Holophrases, speech acts and language universals. Journal of Child Language, 2, 21–40. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fogel, A., & DeKoeyer-Laros, I.
(2007) The developmental transition to secondary intersubjectivity in the second half year: A microgenetic case study. Journal of Developmental Processes, 2, 63–90.Google Scholar
Franco, F., & Butterworth, G.
(1996) Pointing and social awareness; declaring and requesting in the second year. Journal of Child Language, 23, 307–336. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Geert, P. van
(2008) The dynamic systems approach in the study of L1 and L2 acquisition: An introduction. Modern Language Journal, 92, 179–199. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Goldstein, M., & Schwade, J.
(2008) Social feedback to infants’ babbling facilitates rapid phonological learning. Psychological Science, 19, 515–23. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Iverson, J., & Goldin-Meadow, S.
(2005) Gesture paves the way for language development. Psychological Science, 16, 367–371. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Jones, E., & Zimmerman, D.
(2003) A child’s point of view and the achievement of intentionality. Gesture, 3, 155–185. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kaye, K., & Fogel, A.
(1980) The temporal structure of face-to-face communication between mothers and infants. Developmental Psychology, 16, 454–464. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Keller, H.
(2007) Cultures of infancy. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Lieven, E.
(1994) Crosslinguistic and crosscultural aspects of language addressed to children. In C. Gallaway & B. J. Richards (Eds.), Input and interaction in language acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lieven, E. V., Pine, J. M., & Barnes, H. D.
(1992) Individual differences in early vocabulary development: Redefining the referential-expressive distinction. Journal of Child Language, 19(2), 287–310. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lieven, E. V., & Stoll, S.
(2013) Early communicative development in two cultures: A comparison of the communicative environments of children from two cultures. Human Development, 56(3), 178–206. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Liszkowski, U., Brown, P., Callaghan, T., Takada, A., & Vos, C. de.
(2012) A prelinguistic gestural universal of human communication. Cognitive Science, 36, 698–713. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lock, A., & Zukow-Goldring, P.
(2010) Preverbal communication. In J. G. Bremner & T. D. Wachs (Eds.), Handbook of infant development. Oxford: Blackwell. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Luo, R., & Tamis-LeMonda, C.
(2016) Mothers’ verbal and nonverbal strategies in relation to infants’ object-directed actions in real time and across the first three years in ethnically diverse families. Infancy, 21, 65–89. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Masur, E.
(1983) Gestural development, dual-directional signalling, and the transition to words. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 12, 93–109. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Masur, E., Flynn, V., & Eichorst, D.
(2005) Maternal responsive and directive behaviours and utterances as predictors of children’s lexical development. Journal of Child Language, 32, 63–91. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
McGillion, M., Herbert, J., Pine, J., Keren-Portnoy, T., Vihman, M., & Matthews, D.
(2013) Supporting early vocabulary development: what sort of responsiveness matters. IEEE Transactions on Autonomous Mental Development, 5, 240–248. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
McGillion, M., Pine, J., Herbert, J., & Matthews, D.
(2017) A randomised controlled trial to test the effect of promoting caregiver contingent talk on language development in infants from diverse socioeconomic status backgrounds. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 58, 1122–1131. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Moore, C., & Corkum, V.
(1994) Social understanding at the end of the first year of life. Developmental Review, 144, 349–372. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Parisi, D. & Antinucci, F.
(1973) Elementi di grammatica. Torina: Boringheiri.Google Scholar
Pika, S., & Bugnyar, T.
(2011) The use of referential gestures in ravens (Corvus corax) in the wild. Nature Communications, 2, 560. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pye, C.
(1986) An ethnography of Mayan speech to children. Working Papers in Child Language, 1, 30–58.Google Scholar
Reddy, V.
(2010) How infants know minds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Salomo, D., & Liszkowski, U.
(2013) Sociocultural settings influence the emergence of prelinguistic deictic gestures. Child Development, 84, 1296–1307. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Salo, V., Rowe, M., & Reeb-Sutherland, B.
(2018) Exploring infant gesture and joint attention as related constructs and as predictors of later language. Infancy, 23, 432–452. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Schieffelin, B., & Ochs, E.
(1986) Language socialization across cultures. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Searle, J.
(1969) Speech acts: An essay in the philosophy of language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tamis-LeMonda, C., Song, L., Leavell, A., Kahana-Kalman, R., & Yoshikawa, H.
(2012) Ethnic differences in mother–infant language and gestural communications are associated with specific skills in infants. Developmental Science, 15, 384–397. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Thelen, E., & Smith, L.
(1994) A dynamical systems approach to the development of perception and action. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M.
(1999) The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
(2008) Origins of human communication: Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., & Farrar, J.
(1986) Joint attention and early language. Child Development, 57, 1454–1463. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., & Liszkowski, U.
(2007) A new look at infant pointing. Child Development, 78, 705–722. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Trevarthen, C.
(1993) The self born in intersubjectivity: The psychology of an infant communicating. In U. Neisser (Ed.), Emory symposia in cognition, 5. The perceived self: Ecological and interpersonal sources of self-knowledge (pp. 121–173). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Trevarthen, C., & Hubley, P.
(1978) Secondary Intersubjectivity: Confidence, confiding, and acts of meaning in the first year. In J. Lock (Ed.), Action, gesture and symbol (pp. 183–229). New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Triesch, J., Teuscher, C., Deák, G., & Carlson, E.
(2006) Gaze following: why (not) learn it? Developmental Science, 9, 125–147. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Werner, H., & Kaplan, B.
(1963) Symbol formation: An organismic-developmental approach to language and the expression of thought. New York, NY: Wiley.Google Scholar