Chapter published in:
Current Perspectives on Child Language Acquisition: How children use their environment to learn
Edited by Caroline F. Rowland, Anna L. Theakston, Ben Ambridge and Katherine E. Twomey
[Trends in Language Acquisition Research 27] 2020
► pp. 189219
References

References

Abrahams, B. S., Tentler, D., Perederiy, J. V., Oldham, M. C., Coppola, G., & Geschwind, D. H.
(2007) Genome-wide analyses of human perisylvian cerebral cortical patterning. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(45), 17849–17854. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Alarcón, M., Abrahams, B. S., Stone, J. L., Duvall, J. A., Perederiy, J. V., Bomar, J. M., … Geschwind, D. H.
(2008) Linkage, association, and gene-expression analyses identify CNTNAP2 as an autism-susceptibility gene. The American Journal of Human Genetics, 82(1), 150–159. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ambridge, B.
(2018) Against stored abstractions: A radical exemplar model of language acquisition. First Language, Crossref
Ambridge, B., Kidd, E., Rowland, C. F., & Theakston, A. L.
(2015) The ubiquity of frequency effects in first language acquisition. Journal of Child Language, 42(2), 239–273. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bates, E., Bretheron, I., & Snyder, L.
(1988) From first words to grammar: Individual differences and dissociable mechanisms. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bates, E., Dale, P., & Thal, D.
(1995) Individual differences and their implications for theories of language development. In P. Fletcher & B. Macwhinney (Eds.), The handbook of child language (pp. 96–151). Oxford: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Bates, E., & Goodman, J. C.
(1997) On the inseparability of grammar and the lexicon: Evidence from acquisition, aphasia, and real-time processing. Language and Cognitive Processes, 12(5/6), 507–584.Google Scholar
Bates, E., & Macwhinney, B.
(1989) Functionalism and the competition model. In B. MacWhinney & E. Bates (Eds.), The crosslinguistic study of sentence processing (pp. 3–76). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Beals, D. E.
(1997) Sources of support for learning words in conversation: Evidence from mealtimes. Journal of Child Language, 24(3), 673–694. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bishop, D. V. M.
(2003) Test for reception of grammar: TROG-2 version 2. London: Pearson Assessment.Google Scholar
Bleses, D., Vach, W., Slott, M., Wehberg, S., Thomsen, P., Madsen, T. O., & Basbøll, H.
(2008) The Danish communicative developmental inventories: Validity and main developmental trends. Journal of Child Language, 35(03), 651–669. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bloom, L., Lightbown, P., Hood, L., Bowerman, M., Maratsos, M., & Maratsos, M. P.
(1975) Structure and variation in child language. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 40(2). CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bloomfield, F. H.
(2011) Epigenetic modifications may play a role in the developmental consequences of early life events. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders, 3(4), 348–55. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Borer, H., & Wexler, K.
(1987) The maturation of syntax. In T. Roeper & E. Williams (Eds.), Parameter setting (pp. 123–172). Dordrecht: Reidel. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bornstein, M. H., Haynes, M. O., & Painter, K. M.
(1998) Sources of child vocabulary competence: A multivariate model. Journal of Child Language, 25(2), 367–393. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bornstein, M. H., & Putnick, D. L.
(2012) Stability of language in childhood: A multiage, multidomain, multimeasure, and multisource study. Developmental Psychology, 48(2), 477–491. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Borovsky, A., Ellis, E. M., Evans, J. L., & Elman, J. L.
(2016) Semantic structure invocabulary knowledge interacts with lexical and sentence processing in infancy. Child Development, 87(6), 1893–1908. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Brito, N. H., Fifer, W. P., Myers, M. M., Elliott, A. J., & Noble, K. G.
(2016) Associations among family socioeconomic status, EEG power at birth, and cognitive skills during infancy. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 144–151. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Caplan, D., & Waters, G. S.
(1999) Verbal working memory and sentence comprehension. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22(1), 77–94. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Carlson, M. T., Sonderegger, M., & Bane, M.
(2014) How children explore the phonological network in child-directed speech: A survival analysis of children’s first word productions. Journal of Memory and Language, 75, 159–180. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cartmill, E., Armstrong, B. F., Gleitman, L. R., Goldin-Meadows, S., Medinac, T. N., & Trueswell, J. C.
(2013) Quality of early parent input predicts child vocabulary 3 years later. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110(28), 11278–11283. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Chen, Q., & Mirman, D.
(2012) Competition and cooperation among similar representations: Toward a unified account of facilitative and inhibitory effects of lexical neighbors. Psychological Review, 119(2), 417–430. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Chonchaiya, W., Tardif, T., Mai, X., Xu, L., Li, M., Kaciroti, N., … Lozoff, B.
(2013) Developmental trends in auditory processing can provide early predictions of language acquisition in young infants. Developmental Science, 16(2), 159–172. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Choudhury, N., Leppanen, P. H. T., Leevers, H. J., & Benasich, A. A.
(2007) Infant information processing and family history of specific language impairment: Converging evidence for RAP deficits from two paradigms. Developmental Science, 10(2), 213–236. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Colonnesi, C., Stams, G. J. J. M., Koster, I., & Noom, M. J.
(2010) The relation between pointing and language development: A meta-analysis. Developmental Review, 30(4), 352–366. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Conti-Ramsden, G., Botting, N., & Faragher, B.
(2001) Psycholinguistic markers for Specific Language Impairment (SLI). Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42(6), 741–748. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Conti-Ramsden, G., & Hesketh, A.
(2003) Risk markers for SLI: A study of young language-learning children. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 38, 251–263. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cristià, A.
(2011) Fine-grained variation in caregivers’ /s/ predicts their infants’ /s/ category. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 129(5), 3271–80. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cristià, A., Seidl, A., Junge, C., Soderstrom, M., & Hagoort, P.
(2014) Predicting individual variation in language from infant speech perception measures. Child Development, 85(4), 1330–1345. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dąbrowska, E., & Street, J. A.
(2006) Individual differences in language attainment: Comprehension of passive sentences by native and non-native English speakers. Language Sciences, 28(6), 604–615. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Demir-Vegter, S., Aarts, R., & Kurvers, J.
(2014) Lexical richness in maternal input and vocabulary development of Turkish preschoolers in the Netherlands. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 43(2), 149–65. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Donnelly, S., & Kidd, E.
(2020) Individual differences in lexical processing efficiency and vocabulary in toddlers: A longitudinal investigation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 192. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Duyck, W., Desmet, T., Verbeke, L. P. C., & Brysbaert, M.
(2004) WordGen: A tool for word selection and nonword generation in Dutch, English, German, and French. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 36(3), 488–499. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ellis Weismer, S., Tomblin, J. B., Zhang, X., Buckwalter, P., Chynoweth, J. G., & Jones, M.
(2000) Nonword repetition performance in school-age children with and without language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 43(4), 865–78. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Evans, J. L., Saffran, J. R., & Robe-Torres, K.
(2009) Statistical learning in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 52(2), 321–35. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Falcaro, M., Pickles, A., Newbury, D. F., Addis, L., Banfield, E., Fisher, S. E., Monaco, A. P., Simkin, Z., Conti-Ramsden, G. & The SLI Consortium
(2008) Genetic and phenotypic effects of phonological short-term memory and grammatical morphology in specific language impairment. Genes, Brain and Behavior, 7(4), 393–402. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fenson, L., Marchman, V. A., Thal, D. J., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., & Bates, E.
(2007) MacArthur-Bates communicative development inventories: User’s guide and technical manual (2nd ed.). Baltimore, MD: Paul Brookes.Google Scholar
Fernald, A., & Marchman, V. A.
(2012) Individual differences in lexical processing at 18 months predict vocabulary growth in typically developing and late-talking toddlers. Child Development, 83(1), 203–222. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fernald, A., Marchman, V. A., & Weisleder, A.
(2013) SES differences in language processing skill and vocabulary are evident at 18 months. Developmental Science, 16(2), 234–248. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fernald, A., Perfors, A., & Marchman, V. A.
(2006) Picking up speed in understanding: Speech processing efficiency and vocabulary growth across the 2nd year. Developmental Psychology, 42(1), 98–116. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Festman, J., Rodriguez-Fornells, A., & Münte, T. F.
(2010) Individual differences in control of language interference in late bilinguals are mainly related to general executive abilities. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 6(1), 5. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Frank, M. C., Braginsky, M., Marchman, V., & Yurovsky, D.
(2019) Variability and consistency in early language learning. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. Retrieved from https://​langcog​.github​.io​/wordbank​-book​/index​.html> (29 January 2020).
Frank, M. C., Braginsky, M., Yurovsky, D., & Marchman, V. A.
(2017) Wordbank: An open repository for developmental vocabulary data. Journal of Child Language, 44(3), 677–694. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S. E.
(2006) Nonword repetition and word learning: The nature of the relationship. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27, 513–543. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., Willis, C. S., Emslie, H., & Baddeley, A. D.
(1992) Phonological memory and vocabulary development during the early school years: A longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 28(5), 887–898. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Harris, J. R.
(1998) The nuture assumption: Why children turn out the way they do. New York, NY: Free Press.Google Scholar
Hart, B., & Risley, T. R.
(1992) American parenting of language-learning children: Persisting differences in family–child interactions observed in natural home environments. Developmental Psychology, 28(6), 1096–1105. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(1995) Meaningful differences in the everyday experiences of young American children. Baltimore, MD: Paul Brookes.Google Scholar
Hills, T. T., Maouene, M., Maouene, J., Sheya, A., & Smith, L.
(2009) Longitudinal analysis of early semantic networks: preferential attachment or preferential acquisition? Psychological Science, 20(6), 729–39. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hirsh-Pasek, K., Adamson, L. B., Bakeman, R., Owen, M. T., Golinkoff, R. M., Pace, A., … Suma, K.
(2015) The contribution of early communication quality to low-income children’s language success. Psychological Science, 26(7), 1071–1083. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hirsh-Pasek, K., Cauley, K. M., Golinkoff, R. M., & Gordon, L.
(1987) The eyes have it: Lexical and syntactic comprehension in a new paradigm. Journal of Child Language, 14(1), 23–45. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Golinkoff, R. M.
(1996) The origins of grammar: Evidence from early language comprehension. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Hoareau, M., Yeung, H. H., & Nazzi, T.
(2019) Infants’ statistical word segmentation in an artificial language is linked to both parental speech input and reported production abilities. Developmental Science. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hoff, E., Core, C., Place, S., Rumiche, R., Señor, M., & Parra, M.
(2012) Dual language exposure and early bilingual development. Journal of Child Language, 39(1), 1–27. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hoff, E., & Naigles, L.
(2002) How children use input to acquire a lexicon. Child Development, 73(2), 418–433. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hoff, E., Quinn, J. M., & Giguere, D.
(2018) What explains the correlation between growth in vocabulary and grammar? New evidence from latent change score analyses of simultaneous bilingual development. Developmental Science, 21(2), e12536. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hsu, N., Hadley, P. A., & Rispoli, M.
(2017) Diversity matters: Parent input predicts toddler verb production. Journal of Child Language, 44(1), 63–86. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hurtado, N., Marchman, V. A., & Fernald, A.
(2007) Spoken word recognition by Latino children learning Spanish as their first language. Journal of Child Language, 33, 227–249. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2008) Does input influence uptake? Links between maternal talk, processing speed and vocabulary size in Spanish-learning children. Developmental Science, 11(6), F31–F39. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Huttenlocher, J., Haight, W., Bryk, A., Seltzer, M., & Lyons, T.
(1991) Early vocabulary growth: Relation to language input and gender. Developmental Psychology, 27(2), 236–248. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Huttenlocher, J., Waterfall, H., Vasilyeva, M., Vevea, J., & Hedges, L. V.
(2010) Sources of variability in children’s language growth. Cognitive Psychology, 61, 343–365. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hyams, N. M.
(1986) Language acquisition and the theory of parameters. Dordrecht: Reidel. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Iverson, J. M., & Goldin-Meadow, S.
(2005) Gesture paves the way for language development. Psychological Science, 16(5), 367–371. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, W., Turkheimer, E., Gottesman, I. I., & Bouchard, T. J. J.
(2010) Beyond heritability: Twin studies in behavioral research. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18(4), 217–220. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Jones, G., Gobet, F., & Pine, J. M.
(2007) Linking working memory and long-term memory: A computational model of the learning of new words. Developmental Science, 10(6), 853–873. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Jones, G., & Rowland, C. F.
(2017) Diversity not quantity in caregiver speech: Using computational modeling to isolate the effects of the quantity and the diversity of the input on vocabulary growth. Cognitive Psychology, 98, 1–21. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Junge, C., Cutler, A., & Hagoort, P.
(2012) Electrophysiological evidence of early word learning. Neuropsychologia, 50(14), 3702–3712. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A.
(1992) A capacity theory of comprehension: Individual differences in working memory. Psychological Review, 99(1), 122–49. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kail, R., & Salthouse, T. A.
(1994) Processing speed as a mental capacity. Acta Psychologica, 86(2–3), 199–225. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A.
(1998) Development itself is the key to understanding developmental disorders. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2(10), 389–398. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Khanna, M. M., & Boland, J. E.
(2010) Children’s use of language context in lexical ambiguity resolution. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63(1), 160–193. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kaufman, S. B., Deyoung, C. G., Gray, J. R., Jimenez, L., Brown, J. & Mackintosh, N.
(2010) Implicit learning as an ability, Cognition, 116(3), 321–40 CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kidd, E.
(2013) The role of verbal working memory in children’s sentence comprehension. Topics in Language Disorders, 33(3), 208–223. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kidd, E., & Arciuli, J.
(2016) Individual differences in statistical learning predict children’s comprehension of syntax. Child Development, 87(1), 184–193. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kidd, E., Donnelly, S., & Christiansen, M. H.
(2018) Individual differences in language acquisition and processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22(2), 154–169. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kidd, E., Junge, C., Spokes, T., Morrison, L., & Cutler, A.
(2018) Individual differences in infant speech segmentation: Achieving the lexical shift. Infancy, 23(6), 770–794. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kishimoto, T., Shizawa, Y., Yasuda, J., Hinobayashi, T., & Minami, T.
(2007) Do pointing gestures by infants provoke comments from adults? Infant Behavior and Development, 30(4), 562–567. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kovas, Y., Hayiou-Thomas, M. E., Oliver, B., Dale, P. S., Bishop, D. V. M., & Plomin, R.
(2005) Genetic influences in different aspects of language development: The etiology of language skills in 4.5-year-old twins. Child Development, 76(3), 632–651. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kuhl, P. K., Conboy, B. T., Coffey-Corina, S., Padden, D., Rivera-Gaxiola, M., & Nelson, T.
(2008) Phonetic learning as a pathway to language: new data and native language magnet theory expanded (NLM-e). Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 363(1493), 979–1000. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lany, J.
(2018) Lexical-processing efficiency leverages novel word learning in infants and toddlers. Developmental Science, 21(3), e12569. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lany, J., Giglio, M., & Oswald, M.
(2018) Infants’ lexical processing efficiency is related to vocabulary size by one year of age. Infancy, 23(3), 342–366. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Law, J., Schoon, I., & Parsons, S.
(2009) Modeling developmental language difficulties from school entry into adulthood: Literacy, mental health and employment outcomes. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 52(6), 1401–1416. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lieven, E. V. M.
(1994) Crosslinguistic and crosscultural aspects of language addressed to children. In C. Gallaway & B. J. Richards (Eds.), Input and interaction in language acquisition (pp. 56–73). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2016) Usage-based approaches to language development: Where do we go from here? Language and Cognition, 8, 346–368. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Liu, H.-M., Kuhl, P. K., & Tsao, F.-M.
(2003) An association between mothers’ speech clarity and infants’ speech discrimination skills. Developmental Science, 6(3), F1–F10. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Macdonald, M. C.
(2013) How language production shapes language form and comprehension. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 226. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
MacDonald, M. C., & Christiansen, M. H.
(2002) Reassessing working memory: Comment on Just and Carpenter (1992) and Waters and Caplan (1996). Psychological Review, 109(1), 35–54; discussion 55–74. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Mani, N., & Plunkett, K.
(2010) In the infant’s mind’s ear. Psychological Science, 21(7), 908–913. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2011) Phonological priming and cohort effects in toddlers. Cognition, 121(2), 196–206. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Marchman, V. A., & Fernald, A.
(2008) Speed of word recognition and vocabulary knowledge in infancy predict cognitive and language outcomes in later childhood. Developmental Science, 11(3), F9–16. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Masten, A. S., & Cicchetti, D.
(2010) Developmental cascades. Development and Psychopathology, 22(03), 491–495. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Matthews, D., Behne, T., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M.
(2012) Origins of the human pointing gesture: A training study. Developmental Science, 15(6), 817–829. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
McGillion, M. L., Herbert, J. S., Pine, J. M., Keren-Portnoy, T., Vihman, M. M., & Matthews, D. E.
(2013) Supporting early vocabulary development: What sort of responsiveness matters. IEEE Transactions on Autonomous Mental Development, 5(3), 240–248. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
McGillion, M. L., Herbert, J. S., Pine, J., Vihman, M. M., Depaolis, R., Keren-Portnoy, T., & Matthews, D.
(2016) What paves the way to conventional language? The predictive value of babble, pointing and SES. Child Development, 88(1), 156–166. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Montgomery, J. W., Magimairaj, B. M., & O’Malley, M. H.
(2008) Role of working memory in typically developing children’s complex sentence comprehension. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 37(5), 331–354. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, K.
(1973) Structure and strategy in learning to talk. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 38(1–2).Google Scholar
Novick, J. M., Hussey, E., Teubner-Rhodes, S., Harbison, J. I., & Bunting, M. F.
(2014) Clearing the garden-path: Improving sentence processing through cognitive control training. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 29(2), 186–217. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Nozari, N., Trueswell, J. C., & Thompson-Schill, S. L.
(2016) The interplay of local attraction, context and domain-general cognitive control in activation and suppression of semantic distractors during sentence comprehension. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 23(6), 1942–1953. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pan, B. A., Rowe, M. L., Singer, J. D., & Snow, C. E.
(2005) Maternal correlates of growth in toddler vocabulary production in low-income families. Child Development, 76(4), 763–82.Google Scholar
Pearson, B. Z., Fernandez, S. C., Lewedeg, V., & Oller, D. K.
(1997) The relation of input factors to lexical learning by bilingual infants. Applied Psycholinguistics, 18(1), 41–58. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pine, J. M., & Lieven, E. V.
(1993) Reanalysing rote-learned phrases: Individual differences in the transition to multi-word speech. Journal of Child Language, 20(3), 551–571. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pine, J. M., Lieven, E. V., & Rowland, C.
(1996) Observational and checklist measures of vocabulary composition: what do they mean? Journal of Child Language, 23(3), 573–590. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Radford, A.
(1990) Syntactic theory and the acquisition of English syntax: The nature of early grammars of English. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Rämä, P., Sirri, L., & Serres, J.
(2013) Development of lexical–semantic language system: N400 priming effect for spoken words in 18- and 24-month old children. Brain and Language, 125(1), 1–10. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rice, M. L., Wexler, K., & Cleave, P. L.
(1995) Specific language impairment as a period of extended optional infinitive. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38(4), 850–63. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rivera-Gaxiola, M., Silva-Pereyra, J., & Kuhl, P. K.
(2005) Brain potentials to native and non-native speech contrasts in 7- and 11-month-old American infants. Developmental Science, 8(2), 162–72. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Romeo, R. R., Leonard, J. A., Robinson, S. T., West, M. R., Mackey, A. P., Rowe, M. L., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. E.
(2018) Beyond the 30-million-word gap: Children’s conversational exposure is associated with language-related brain function. Psychological Science, 29(5), 700–710. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Roth, C., Magnus, P., Schjølberg, S., Stoltenberg, C., Surén, P., McKeague, I. W., … Susser, E.
(2011) Folic acid supplements in pregnancy and severe language delay in children. JAMA, 306(14), 1566. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rowe, M. L.
(2012) A longitudinal investigation of the role of quantity and quality of child-directed speech in vocabulary development. Child Development, 83(5), 1762–1774. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Saffran, J. R., Aslin, R. N., & Newport, E. L.
(1996) Statistical learning by 8-month-old infants. Science, 274, 1926–1928. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Seidenberg, M. S., & MacDonald, M. C.
(1999) A probabilistic constraints approach to language acquisition and processing. Cognitive Science, 23(4), 569–588. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Skeide, M. A., & Friederici, A. D.
(2016) The ontogeny of the cortical language network. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 17(5), 323–332. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Spearman, C.
(1904) “General Intelligence,” objectively determined and measured. The American Journal of Psychology, 15(2), 201–293. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Stokes, S. F., Wong, A. M.-Y., Fletcher, P., & Leonard, L. B.
(2006) Nonword repetition and sentence repetition as clinical markers of Specific Language Impairment: The case of Cantonese. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49(2), 219–236. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Street, J. A., & Dąbrowska, E.
(2010) More individual differences in language attainment: How much do adult native speakers of English know about passives and quantifiers? Lingua, 12(8), 2080–2094. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2014) Lexically specific knowledge and individual differences in adult native speakers’ processing of the English passive. Applied Psycholinguistics, 35(1), 97–118. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M.
(2001) First steps toward a usage-based theory of language acquisition. Cognitive Linguistics, 11, 61–82. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tsao, F. M., Liu, H. M., & Kuhl, P. K.
(2004) Speech perception in infancy predicts language development in the second year of life: A longitudinal study. Child Development, 75(4), 1067–1084. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Van der Lely, H. K. J., & Pinker, S.
(2014) The biological basis of language: Insight from developmental grammatical impairments. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(11), 586–595. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Vernes, S. C., Newbury, D. F., Abrahams, B. S., Winchester, L., Nicod, J., Groszer, M., … Fisher, S. E.
(2008) A functional genetic link between distinct developmental language disorders. New England Journal of Medicine, 359(22), 2337–2345. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Vuong, L. C., & Martin, R. C.
(2014) Domain-specific executive control and the revision of misinterpretations in sentence comprehension. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 29(3), 312–325. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Weisleder, A., & Fernald, A.
(2013) Talking to children matters: Early language experience strengthens processing and builds vocabulary. Psychological Science, 24(11), 2143–2152. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Weizman, Z. O., & Snow, C. E.
(2001) Lexical input as related to children’s vocabulary acquisition: Effects of sophisticated exposure and support for meaning. Developmental Psychology, 37(2), 265–279. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wellman, H.
(2014) Making minds: How theory of mind develops. Oxford: Oxford University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wells, J. B., Christiansen, M. H., Race, D. S., Acheson, D. J., & MacDonald, M. C.
(2009) Experience and sentence processing: Statistical learning and relative clause comprehension. Cognitive Psychology, 58(2), 250–271. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Whitehouse, A. J. O., Bishop, D. V. M., Ang, Q. W., Pennell, C. E., & Fisher, S. E.
(2011) CNTNAP2 variants affect early language development in the general population. Genes, Brain and Behavior, 10(4), 451–456. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Willoughby, M. T., Blair, C. B., & Family Life Project Investigators, the F. L. P.
(2016) Measuring executive function in early childhood: A case for formative measurement. Psychological Assessment, 28(3), 319–30. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wojcik, E. H.
(2018) The development of lexical-semantic networks in infants and toddlers. Child Development Perspectives, 12(1), 34–38. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Woodard, K., Pozzan, L., & Trueswell, J. C.
(2016) Taking your own path: Individual differences in executive function and language processing skills in child learners. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 141, 187–209. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Yang, C., Crain, S., Berwick, R. C., Chomsky, N., & Bolhuis, J. J.
(2017) The growth of language: Universal Grammar, experience, and principles of computation. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 81, 103–119. CrossrefGoogle Scholar