References
Abbot-Smith, K., & Behrens, H.
(2006) How known constructions influence the acquisition of other constructions: The German passive and future constructions. Cognitive Science 30 (6), 995–1026. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Alcock, K. J., Rimba, K., & Newton, C. R.
(2012) Early production of the passive in two Eastern Bantu languages. First Language, 32 (4), 459–478. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Allen, S. E., & Crago, M. B.
(1996) Early passive acquisition in Inuktitut. Journal of Child Language, 23 (1), 129–155. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ambridge, B.
(2019) Against stored abstractions: A radical exemplar model of language acquisition. First Language. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ambridge, B., Kidd, E., Rowland, C. F., & Theakston, A. L.
(2015) The ubiquity of frequency effects in first language acquisition. Journal of Child Language, 42 (2), 239–273. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, S. C.
(1983) Tone and morpheme rules in Bamileke-Ngyemboon (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Southern California.
Arai, M., & Mazuka, R.
(2014) The development of Japanese passive syntax as indexed by structural priming in comprehension. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67 (1), 60–78. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Barnes, S., Gutfreund, M., Satterly, D., & Wells, G.
(1983) Characteristics of adult speech which predict children’s language development. Journal of Child Language, 10 (1), 65–84. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bencini, G. M., & Valian, V. V.
(2008) Abstract sentence representations in 3-year-olds: Evidence from language production and comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 59 (1), 97–113. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bernolet, S., Colleman, T., & Hartsuiker, R. J.
(2014) The “sense boost” to dative priming: Evidence for sense-specific verb-structure links. Journal of Memory and Language, 76 , 113–126. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bernolet, S., Hartsuiker, R. J., & Pickering, M. J.
(2007) Shared syntactic representations in bilinguals: Evidence for the role of word-order repetition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33 (5), 931–949.Google Scholar
(2013) From language-specific to shared syntactic representations: The influence of second language proficiency on syntactic sharing in bilinguals. Cognition, 127 (3), 287–306. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bock, J. K.
(1986) Syntactic persistence in language production. Cognitive Psychology, 18 (3), 355–387. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(1989) Closed-class immanence in sentence production. Cognition, 31 (2), 163–186. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bock, K., Dell, G. S., Chang, F., & Onishi, K. H.
(2007) Persistent structural priming from language comprehension to language production. Cognition, 104 (3), 437–458. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bock, K., & Loebell, H.
(1990) Framing sentences. Cognition, 35 (1), 1–39. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Branigan, H.
(2007) Syntactic priming. Language and Linguistics Compass, 1 (1–2), 1–16. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Branigan, H. P., & McLean, J. F.
(2016) What children learn from adults’ utterances: An ephemeral lexical boost and persistent syntactic priming in adult–child dialogue. Journal of Memory and Language, 91 , 141–157. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Branigan, H. P., & Messenger, K.
(2016) Consistent and cumulative effects of syntactic experience in children’s sentence production: Evidence for error-based implicit learning. Cognition, 157 , 250–256. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Branigan, H. P., & McLean, J. F.
(2016) What children learn from adults’ utterances: An ephemeral lexical boost and persistent syntactic priming in adult-child dialogue. Journal of Memory and Language, 91 , 141–157. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Branigan, H. P., & Pickering, M. J.
(2017) An experimental approach to linguistic representation. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 40 , Article e282. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Branigan, H. P., Pickering, M. J., Liversedge, S. P., Stewart, A. J., & Urbach, T. P.
(1995) Syntactic priming: Investigating the mental representation of language. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 24 , 489–506. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Buckle, L., Lieven, E., & Theakston, A. L.
(2017) The effects of animacy and syntax on priming: A developmental study. Frontiers in Psychology, 8 , 02246. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bulté, B., & Housen, A.
(2014) Conceptualizing and measuring short-term changes in L2 writing complexity. Journal of Second Language Writing, 26 , 42–65. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Chang, F., Baumann, M., Pappert, S., & Fitz, H.
(2015) Do lemmas speak German? A verb position effect in German structural priming. Cognitive Science, 39 (5), 1113–1130. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Chang, F., Dell, G. S., & Bock, K.
(2006) Becoming syntactic. Psychological Review, 113 (2), 234–272. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Chang, F., & Fitz, H.
(2014) Computational models of sentence production: A dual-path approach. In M. A. Goldrick, V. S. Ferreira, & M. Miozzo (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of language production (pp. 70–87). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Chang, F., Janciauskas, M., & Fitz, H.
(2012) Language adaptation and learning: Getting explicit about implicit learning. Language and Linguistics Compass, 6 (5), 259–278. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Chen, B., Jia, Y., Wang, Z., Dunlap, S., & Shin, J. A.
(2013) Is word-order similarity necessary for cross-linguistic structural priming? Second Language Research, 29 (4), 375–389. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cleland, A. A., & Pickering, M. J.
(2003) The use of lexical and syntactic information in language production: Evidence from the priming of noun-phrase structure. Journal of Memory and Language, 49 (2), 214–230. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Crossley, S. A., Louwerse, M. M., McCarthy, P. M., & McNamara, D. S.
(2007) A linguistic analysis of simplified and authentic texts. Modern Language Journal, 91 (1), 15–30. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dahl, Ö.
(2004) The growth and maintenance of linguistic complexity. John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
De Clercq, B., & Housen, A.
(2017) A cross-linguistic perspective on syntactic complexity in L2 development: Syntactic elaboration and diversity. The Modern Language Journal, 101 (2), 315–334. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
DeGraff, M.
(2003) Against creole exceptionalism. Language 79 (2): 391–410. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dyakonova, M.
(2004) Information structure development: Evidence from the acquisition of word order in Russian and English. Nordlyd, 32 (1): 88–109.Google Scholar
Fenk-Oczlon, G., & Fenk, A.
(2008) Complexity trade-offs between the subsystems of language. In M. Miestamo, K. Sinnemäki, & F. Karlsson (Eds.), Language complexity: Typology, contact, change (pp. 43–65). John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Bates, E., Thal, D. J., & Pethick, S. J.
(1994) Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59 (5), 1–185. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ferreira, V. S., & Bock, K.
(2006) The functions of structural priming. Language and Cognitive Processes, 21 (7–8), 1011–1029. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fine, A. B., & Jaeger, T. F.
(2016) The role of verb repetition in cumulative structural priming in comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 42 (9), 1362–1376.Google Scholar
Fisher, C.
(2002) The role of abstract syntactic knowledge in language acquisition: A reply to Tomasello (2000). Cognition, 82 , 259–278. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fleischer, Z., Pickering, M. J., & McLean, J. F.
(2012) Shared information structure: Evidence from cross-linguistic priming. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15 (3), 568–579. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Foltz, A., Gaspers, J., Meyer, C., Thiele, K., Cimiano, P., & Stenneken, P.
(2015) Temporal effects of alignment in text-based, task-oriented discourse. Discourse Processes, 52 (8), 609–641. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Foltz, A., Knopf, K., Jonas, K., Jaecks, P., & Stenneken, P.
(2020) Evidence for robust abstract syntactic representations in production before age three. First Language. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Foltz, A., Thiele, K., Kahsnitz, D., & Stenneken, P.
(2015) Children’s syntactic-priming magnitude: Lexical factors and participant characteristics. Journal of Child Language, 42 (4), 932–945. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Furrow, D., Nelson, K., & Benedict, H.
(1979) Mothers’ speech to children and syntactic development: Some simple relationships. Journal of Child Language, 6 (3), 423–442. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gámez, P. B., & Shimpi, P. M.
(2016) Structural priming in Spanish as evidence of implicit learning. Journal of Child Language, 43 (1), 207–233. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gámez, P. B., Shimpi, P. M., Waterfall, H. R., & Huttenlocher, J.
(2009) Priming a perspective in Spanish monolingual children: The use of syntactic alternatives. Journal of Child Language, 36 (2), 269–290. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gámez, P. B., & Vasilyeva, M.
(2015) Increasing second language learners’ production and comprehension of developmentally advanced syntactic forms. Language Learning and Development, 11 (2), 128–151. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gertner, Y., Fisher, C., & Eisengart, J.
(2006) Learning words and rules: Abstract knowledge of word order in early sentence comprehension. Psychological Science, 17 (8), 684–691. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gil, D.
(2008) The acquisition of voice morphology in Jakarta Indonesian. In N. Gagarina & I. Gülzow (Eds.), The acquisition of verbs and their grammar: The effect of particular languages (pp. 201–227). Springer. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hall, M. L., Ferreira, V. S., & Mayberry, R. I.
(2015) Syntactic priming in American Sign Language. PLOS ONE, 10 (3), e0119611. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hartsuiker, R. J., Bernolet, S., Schoonbaert, S., Speybroeck, S., & Vanderelst, D.
(2008) Syntactic priming persists while the lexical boost decays: Evidence from written and spoken dialogue. Journal of Memory and Language, 58 (2), 214–238. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hartsuiker, R. J., Pickering, M. J., & Veltkamp, E.
(2004) Is syntax separate or shared between languages? Cross-linguistic syntactic priming in Spanish-English bilinguals. Psychological Science, 15 (6), 409–414. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hartsuiker, R. J., & Westenberg, C.
(2000) Word order priming in written and spoken sentence production. Cognition, 75 (2), B27–B39. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hoff-Ginsberg, E.
(1985) Some contributions of mothers’ speech to their children’s syntactic growth. Journal of Child Language, 12 (2), 367–385. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Holst, J. H.
(2014) Advances in Burushaski linguistics. BoD–Books on Demand.Google Scholar
Horgan, D.
(1978) The development of the full passive. Journal of Child Language, 5 , 65–80. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hsin, L., Legendre, G., & Omaki, A.
(2013) Priming cross-linguistic interference in Spanish–English bilingual children. In S. Baiz, N. Goldman, & R. Hawks (Eds.), BUCLD 37: Proceedings of the 37th annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 165–177). Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Hujanen, J. H. H.
(1997) Effects of case marking and word order on sentence parsing in Finnish: An eye fixation analysis. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Section A, 50 (4), 841–858. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Huttenlocher, J., Vasilyeva, M., Cymerman, E., & Levine, S.
(2002) Language input and child syntax. Cognitive Psychology, 45 (3), 337–374. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Huttenlocher, J., Vasilyeva, M., & Shimpi, P.
(2004) Syntactic priming in young children. Journal of Memory and Language, 50 (2), 182–195. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Jaeger, T. F., & Snider, N. E.
(2013) Alignment as a consequence of expectation adaptation: Syntactic priming is affected by the prime’s prediction error given both prior and recent experience. Cognition, 127 (1), 57–83. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Jisa, H., Reilly, J., Verhoeven, L., Baruch, E., & Rosado, E.
(2002) Passive voice constructions in written texts: A cross-linguistic developmental study. Written Language and Literacy, 5 (2), 163–181. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Juola, P.
(1998) Measuring linguistic complexity: The morphological tier. Journal of Quantitative Linguistics, 5 (3), 206–213. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kaschak, M. P.
(2007) Long-term structural priming affects subsequent patterns of language production. Memory & Cognition, 35 (5), 925–937. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kaschak, M. P., Kutta, T. J., & Schatschneider, C.
(2011) Long-term cumulative structural priming persists for (at least) one week. Memory & Cognition, 39 (3), 381–388. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kaschak, M. P., Loney, R. A., & Borreggine, K. L.
(2006) Recent experience affects the strength of structural priming. Cognition, 99 (3), B73–B82. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kidd, E.
(2012a) Implicit statistical learning is directly associated with the acquisition of syntax. Developmental Psychology, 48 (1), 171–184. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2012b) Individual differences in syntactic priming in language acquisition. Applied Psycholinguistics, 33 , 393–418. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kline, M., & Demuth, K.
(2010) Factors facilitating implicit learning: The case of the Sesotho passive. Language Acquisition, 17 (4), 220–234. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kuiken, F., Vedder, I., Housen, A., & De Clercq, B.
(2019) Variation in syntactic complexity: Introduction. International Journal of Applied Linguistics, 29 (2), 161–170. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Leonard, L. B., Miller, C. A., Grela, B., Holland, A. L., Gerber, E., & Petucci, M.
(2000) Production operations contribute to the grammatical morpheme limitations of children with specific language impairment. Journal of Memory and Language, 43 (2), 362–378. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lin, J. W.
(2006) Time in a language without tense: The case of Chinese. Journal of Semantics, 23 (1), 1–53. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Loban, W.
(1976) Language development: Kindergarten through grade twelve. National Council of Teachers of English.Google Scholar
McWhorter, J. H.
(2001) The worlds simplest grammars are creole grammars. Linguistic Typology, 5 (2–3), 125–166. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2011) Linguistic simplicity and complexity: Why do languages undress? Walter de Gruyter. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Messenger, K.
(2021) The persistence of priming: Exploring long-lasting syntactic priming effects in children and adults. Cognitive Science, 45 (6), e13005. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Messenger, K., Branigan, H. P., & McLean, J. F.
(2011) Evidence for (shared) abstract structure underlying children’s short and full passives. Cognition, 121 , 268–274. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Messenger, K., Branigan, H. P., McLean, J. F., & Sorace, A.
(2012) Is young children’s passive syntax semantically constrained? Evidence from syntactic priming. Journal of Memory and Language, 66 , 568–587. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Meyer, A. S.
(2017) Structural priming is not a royal road to representations. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 40 , e305. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Miestamo, M.
(2008) Grammatical complexity in a cross-linguistic perspective. In M. Miestamo, K. Sinnemäki, & F. Karlsson (Eds.), Language complexity: Typology, contact, change (pp. 23–41). John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Miller, J. F., & Chapman, R. S.
(1981) The relation between age and mean length of utterance. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 24 (2), 154–161. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Muylle, M., Bernolet, S., & Hartsuiker, R. J.
(2020) The role of case marking and word order in cross-linguistic structural priming in late L2 acquisition. Language Learning, 70 (S2), 194–220. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Naigles, L. R.
(2002) Form is easy, meaning is hard: Resolving a paradox in early child language. Cognition, 86 (2), 157–199. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Norris, J. M., & Ortega, L.
(2009) Towards an organic approach to investigating CAF in instructed SLA: The case of complexity. Applied Linguistics, 30 (4), 555–578. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Parkvall, M.
(2008) The simplicity of creoles in a cross-linguistic perspective. In M. Miestamo, K. Sinnemäki, & F. Karlsson (Eds.), Language complexity: Typology, contact, change (pp. 265–285). John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pasch, H.
(1997) Sango. In S. G. Thomason (Ed.), Contact languages: A wider perspective (pp. 209–270). John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Perek, F.
(2012) Alternation-based generalizations are stored in the mental grammar: Evidence from a sorting task experiment. Cognitive Linguistics, 23 (3), 601–635. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Peter, M., Chang, F., Pine, J. M., Blything, R., & Rowland, C. F.
(2015) When and how do children develop knowledge of verb argument structure? Evidence from verb bias effects in a structural priming task. Journal of Memory and Language, 81 , 1–15. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pickering, M. J., & Branigan, H. P.
(1998) The representation of verbs: Evidence from syntactic priming in language production. Journal of Memory and language, 39 (4), 633–651. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(1999) Syntactic priming in language production. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3 (4), 136–141. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pinker, S.
(1989) Learnability and cognition: The acquisition of argument structure. The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Pye, C., & Quixtan Poz, P.
(1988) Precocious passives (and antipassives) in Quiché Mayan. Papers and Reports on Child Language Development, 27 , 71–80.Google Scholar
Rissman, L., Legendre, G., & Landau, B.
(2013) Abstract morphosyntax in two-and three-year-old children: Evidence from priming. Language Learning and Development, 9 (3), 278–292. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rowland, C. F., Chang, F., Ambridge, B., Pine, J. M., & Lieven, E. V.
(2012) The development of abstract syntax: Evidence from structural priming and the lexical boost. Cognition, 125 (1), 49–63. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sampson, G., Gil, D., & Trudgill, P.
(Eds.) (2009) Language complexity as an evolving variable. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Sarilar, A., Matthews, D., & Küntay, A. C.
(2015) Hearing relative clauses boosts relative clause usage (and referential clarity) in young Turkish language learners. Applied Psycholinguistics, 36 (2), 175–202. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Savage, C., Lieven, E., Theakston, A., & Tomasello, M.
(2003) Testing the abstractness of children’s linguistic representations: Lexical and structural priming of syntactic constructions in young children. Developmental Science, 6 (5), 557–567. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2006) Structural priming as implicit learning in language acquisition: The persistence of lexical and structural priming in 4-year-olds. Language Learning and Development, 2 (1), 27–49. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Scarborough, H., & Wycoff, J.
(1986) Mother, I’d still rather do it myself: Some further non-effects of ‘motherese’. Journal of Child Language, 13 (2), 431–437. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Schoonbaert, S., Hartsuiker, R. J., & Pickering, M. J.
(2007) The representation of lexical and syntactic information in bilinguals: Evidence from syntactic priming. Journal of Memory and Language, 56 (2), 153–171. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Scott, C. M.
(1984) Adverbial connectivity in conversations of children 6–12. Journal of Child Language, 11 (2), 423–452. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Seabrooks, D.
(2017) Child acquisition of Spanish clitic impersonal constructions: An empirical study on the CHILDES corpora (Unpublished MA dissertation). University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill.
Shimpi, P. M., Gámez, P. B., Huttenlocher, J., & Vasilyeva, M.
(2007) Syntactic priming in 3-and 4-year-old children: Evidence for abstract representations of transitive and dative forms. Developmental Psychology, 43 (6), 1334–1346. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Shin, J. A., & Christianson, K.
(2009) Syntactic processing in Korean–English bilingual production: Evidence from cross-linguistic structural priming. Cognition, 112 (1), 175–180. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Shosted, R. K.
(2006) Correlating complexity: A typological approach. Linguistic Typology, 10 (1), 1–40. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sinnemäki, K.
(2008) Complexity trade-offs in core argument marking. In M. Miestamo, K. Sinnemäki, & F. Karlsson (Eds.), Language complexity: Typology, contact, change (pp. 67–88). John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sorace, A., & Serratrice, L.
(2009) Internal and external interfaces in bilingual language development: Beyond structural overlap. International Journal of Bilingualism, 13 (2), 195–210. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Szmrecsányi, B.
(2004) On operationalizing syntactic complexity. In Le poids des mots. Proceedings of the 7th International Conference on Textual Data Statistical Analysis. Louvain-la-Neuve (Vol. 2, pp. 1032–1039). Presses universitaires de Louvain.Google Scholar
Tamburelli, M., Sanoudaki, E., Jones, G., & Sowinska, M.
(2015) Acceleration in the bilingual acquisition of phonological structure: Evidence from Polish–English bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18 (4), 713–725. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Thornton, R.
(2016, December 22). Children’s acquisition of syntactic knowledge. Oxford Research encyclopedia of linguistics. Oxford University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M.
(1992) First verbs: A case study of early grammatical development. Cambridge University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2000) Do young children have adult syntactic competence? Cognition, 74 , 209–253. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2003) Constructing a language: A usage-based theory of language acquisition. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Tooley, K. M., & Traxler, M. J.
(2010) Syntactic priming effects in comprehension: A critical review. Language and Linguistics Compass, 4 (10), 925–937. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2018) Implicit learning of structure occurs in parallel with lexically-mediated syntactic priming effects in sentence comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 98 , 59–76. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ullman, M. T.
(2001) The declarative/procedural model of lexicon and grammar. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 30 (1), 37–69. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Valian, V.
(1999) Input and language acquisition. In W. Ritchie & T. K. Bhatia (Eds.), Handbook of child language acquisition (pp. 497–530). Academic Press.Google Scholar
Vasilyeva, M., & Gámez, P. B.
(2015) Exploring interactions between semantic and syntactic processes: The role of animacy in syntactic priming. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 138 , 15–30. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Vasilyeva, M., Huttenlocher, J., & Waterfall, H.
(2006) Effects of language intervention on syntactic skill levels in preschoolers. Developmental Psychology, 42 (1), 164–174. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Vasilyeva, M., & Waterfall, H.
(2012) Beyond syntactic priming: Evidence for activation of alternative syntactic structures. Journal of Child Language, 39 (2), 258–283. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Vasilyeva, M., Waterfall, H., Gámez, P. B., Gómez, L. E., Bowers, E., & Shimpi, P.
(2010) Cross-linguistic syntactic priming in bilingual children. Journal of Child Language, 37 (5), 1047–1064. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wexler, K.
(1998) Very early parameter setting and the unique checking constraint: A new explanation of the optional infinitive stage. Lingua, 106 , 23–79. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Whitehurst, G. J., Ironsmith, M., & Goldfein, M.
(1974) Selective imitation of the passive construction through modeling. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 17 (2), 288–302. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ziegler, J., Snedeker, J., & Wittenberg, E.
(2018) Event structures drive semantic structural priming, not thematic roles: Evidence from idioms and light verbs. Cognitive Science, 42 (8), 2918–2949. CrossrefGoogle Scholar