References
Allen, M. L., Haywood, S., Rajendran, G., & Branigan, H.
(2011) Evidence for syntactic alignment in children with autism. Developmental Science, 14(3), 540–548. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Armon-Lotem, S., Haman, E., Jensen de López, K., Smoczynska, M., Yatsushiro, K., Szczerbinski, M., … van der Lely, H.
(2016) A large-scale cross-linguistic investigation of the acquisition of passive. Language Acquisition, 23(1), 27–56. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bencini, G. M. L., & Valian, V. V.
(2008) Abstract sentence representations in 3-year-olds: Evidence from language production and comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 59(1), 97–113. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bock, K., & Griffin, Z. M.
(2000) The persistence of structural priming: Transient activation or implicit learning? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129(2), 177–192. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Borovsky, A., Elman, J. L., & Fernald, A.
(2012) Knowing a lot for one’s age: Vocabulary skill and not age is associated with anticipatory incremental sentence interpretation in children and adults. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 112(4), 417–436. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Branigan, H. P., & McLean, J. F.
(2016) What children learn from adults’ utterances: An ephemeral lexical boost and persistent syntactic priming in adult–child dialogue. Journal of Memory and Language, 91, 141–157. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Branigan, H. P., McLean, J. F., & Jones, M.
(2005) A blue cat or a cat that is blue? Evidence for abstract syntax in young children’s noun phrases. In A. Brugos, M. Clark-Cotton, & S. Ha (Eds.), BUCLD 29: The proceedings of the twenty-ninth annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 109–121). Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Branigan, H. P., & Messenger, K.
(2016) Consistent and cumulative effects of syntactic experience in children’s sentence production: Evidence for error-based implicit learning. Cognition, 157, 250–256. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Branigan, H. P., & Pickering, M. J.
(2017) Structural priming and the representation of language. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 40, e313. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Branigan, H. P., Pickering, M. J., Liversedge, S. P., Stewart, A. J., & Urbach, T. P.
(1995) Syntactic priming: Investigating the mental representation of language. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 24, 489–506. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Buckle, L., Lieven, E., & Theakston, A. L.
(2017) The effects of animacy and syntax on priming: A developmental study. Frontiers in Psychology. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chang, F., Dell, G. S., & Bock, K.
(2006) Becoming syntactic. Psychological Review, 113(2), 234–272. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chang, F., Janciauskas, M., & Fitz, H.
(2012) Language adaptation and learning: Getting explicit about implicit learning. Language and Linguistics Compass, 6/5, 259–278. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dell, G. S., & Chang, F.
(2014) The P-chain: Relating sentence production and its disorders to comprehension and acquisition. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 369(1634), 20120394–20120394. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Demuth, K., Moloi, F., & Machobane, M.
(2010) 3-Year-olds’ comprehension, production, and generalization of Sesotho passives. Cognition, 115(2), 238–251. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fazekas, J., Jessop, A., Pine, J., & Rowland, C.
(2020) Do children learn from their prediction mistakes? A registered report evaluating error-based theories of language acquisition. Royal Society Open Science, 7(11), 180877. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Foltz, A., Knopf, K., Jonas, K., Jaecks, P., & Stenneken, P.
(2020) Evidence for robust abstract syntactic representations in production before age three. First Language, 41(1), 3–20. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Foltz, A., Thiele, K., Kahsnitz, D., & Stenneken, P.
(2015) Children’s syntactic-priming magnitude: Lexical factors and participant characteristics. Journal of Child Language, 42(4), 932–945. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gambi, C., Pickering, M. J., & Rabagliati, H.
(2016) Beyond associations: Sensitivity to structure in pre-schoolers’ linguistic predictions. Cognition, 157, 340–351. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gámez, P. B., & Shimpi, P. M.
(2016) Structural priming in Spanish as evidence of implicit learning. Journal of Child Language, 43(1), 207–233. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gámez, P. B., Shimpi, P. M., Waterfall, H. R., & Huttenlocher, J.
(2009) Priming a perspective in Spanish monolingual children: The use of syntactic alternatives. Journal of Child Language, 36(2), 269–290. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Garraffa, M., Coco, M. I., & Branigan, H. P.
(2015) Effects of immediate and cumulative syntactic experience in language impairment: Evidence from priming of subject relatives in children with SLI. Language Learning and Development, 11(1), 18–40. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gertner, Y., Fisher, C., & Eisengart, J.
(2006) Learning words and rules: Abstract knowledge of word order in early sentence comprehension. Psychological Science, 17(8), 684–691. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hartsuiker, R. J., Bernolet, S., Schoonbaert, S., Speybroeck, S., & Vanderelst, D.
(2008) Syntactic priming persists while the lexical boost decays: Evidence from written and spoken dialogue. Journal of Memory and Language, 58(2), 214–238. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hesketh, A., Serratrice, L., & Ashworth, R.
(2016) Encouraging use of subordination in children’s narratives: A classroom-based priming study. Language Learning and Development, 12(4), 413–428. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hopman, E. W. M., & MacDonald, M. C.
(2018) Production practice during language learning improves comprehension. Psychological Science. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hsu, D.-B.
(2014a) Mandarin-speaking three-year-olds’ demonstration of productive knowledge of syntax: evidence from syntactic productivity and structural priming with the SVO-ba alternation. Journal of Child Language, 41(5), 1115–1146. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2014b) Structural priming as learning: Evidence from Mandarin-learning 5-year-olds. Language Acquisition, 21(2), 156–172. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2019) Children’s adaption to input change using an abstract syntactic representation: Evidence from structural priming in Mandarin-speaking preschoolers. Frontiers in Psychology. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hsu, H. J., & Bishop, D. V. M.
(2014) Sequence-specific procedural learning deficits in children with specific language impairment. Developmental Science, 17(3), 352–365. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hurtado, I., & Montrul, S.
(2021) Priming dative clitics in spoken Spanish as a second and heritage language. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 43(4), 729–752. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Huttenlocher, J., Vasilyeva, M., Cymerman, E., & Levine, S.
(2002) Language input and child syntax. Cognitive Psychology, 45(3), 337–374. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Huttenlocher, J., Vasilyeva, M., & Shimpi, P.
(2004) Syntactic priming in young children. Journal of Memory and Language, 50(2), 182–195. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Huttenlocher, J., Waterfall, H., Vasilyeva, M., Vevea, J., & Hedges, L. V.
(2010) Sources of variability in children’s language growth. Cognitive Psychology, 61(4), 343–365. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ibbotson, P., Theakston, A., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M.
(2010) The role of pronoun frames in early comprehension of transitive constructions in English. Language Learning and Development, 7(1), 24–39. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jaeger, T. F., & Snider, N. E.
(2013) Alignment as a consequence of expectation adaptation: Syntactic priming is affected by the prime’s prediction error given both prior and recent experience. Cognition, 127(1), 57–83. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Janciauskas, M., & Chang, F.
(2018) Input and age-dependent variation in second language learning: A connectionist account. Cognitive Science, 42(S2), 519–554. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kaschak, M. P.
(2007) Long-term structural priming affects subsequent patterns of language production. Memory & Cognition, 35(5), 925–937. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kaschak, M. P., Kutta, T. J., & Schatschneider, C.
(2011) Long-term cumulative structural priming persists for (at least) one week. Memory and Cognition, 39(3), 381–388. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kaschak, M. P., Loney, R. A., & Borreggine, K. L.
(2006) Recent experience affects the strength of structural priming. Cognition, 99(3). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kidd, E.
(2012a) Implicit statistical learning is directly associated with the acquisition of syntax. Developmental Psychology, 48(1), 171–184. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2012b) Individual differences in syntactic priming in language acquisition. Applied Psycholinguistics, 33(2), 393–418. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Krok, W. C., & Leonard, L. B.
(2018) Verb variability and morphosyntactic priming with typically developing 2- and 3-year-olds. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 61(12), 2996–3009. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kutta, T. J., & Kaschak, M. P.
(2012) Changes in task-extrinsic context do not affect the persistence of long-term cumulative structural priming. Acta Psychologica, 141(3), 408–414. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Levelt, W. J. M.
(1989) Speech: From intention to articulation. The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Lew-Williams, C., & Fernald, A.
(2007) Young children learning Spanish make rapid use of grammatical gender in spoken word recognition. Psychological Science, 18(3), 193–198. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lukyanenko, C., & Fisher, C.
(2016) Where are the cookies? Two- and three-year-olds use number-marked verbs to anticipate upcoming nouns. Cognition, 146, 349–370. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Malhotra, G., Pickering, M. J., Branigan, H. P., & Bednar, J. A.
(2008) On the persistence of structural priming: Mechanisms of decay and influence of word-forms. In B. C. Love, K. McRae, & V. M. Sloutsky (Eds.), Proceedings of the 30th annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, Austin, TX (pp. 657–662). Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Mani, N., & Huettig, F.
(2012) Prediction during language processing is a piece of cake – but only for skilled producers. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 38(4), 843–847. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Marchman, V. A., Bates, E., Burkardt, A., & Good, A. B.
(1991) Functional constraints of the acquisition of the passive: Toward a model of the competence to perform. First Language, 11(31), 65–92. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Messenger, K.
(2021) The persistence of priming: Exploring long-lasting syntactic priming effects in children and adults. Cognitive Science, 45(6), e13005. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Messenger, K., Branigan, H. P., & McLean, J. F.
(2011) Evidence for (shared) abstract structure underlying children’s short and full passives. Cognition, 121(2), 268–274. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2012) Is children’s acquisition of the passive a staged process? Evidence from six-and nine-year-olds’ production of passives. Journal of Child Language, 39(5), 991–1016. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Messenger, K., Branigan, H. P., McLean, J. F., & Sorace, A.
(2012) Is young children’s passive syntax semantically constrained? Evidence from syntactic priming. Journal of Memory and Language, 66(4), 568–587. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Messenger, K., & Fisher, C.
(2018) Mistakes weren’t made: Three-year-olds’ comprehension of novel-verb passives provides evidence for early abstract syntax. Cognition, 178, 118–132. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Peter, M., Chang, F., Pine, J. M., Blything, R., & Rowland, C. F.
(2015) When and how do children develop knowledge of verb argument structure? Evidence from verb bias effects in a structural priming task. Journal of Memory and Language, 81, 1–15. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Peter, M., & Rowland, C. F.
(2018) Aligning developmental and processing accounts of implicit and statistical learning. Topics in Cognitive Science, 11(3), 555–572. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pickering, M. J., & Branigan, H. P.
(1998) The representation of verbs: Evidence from syntactic priming in language production. Journal of Memory and Language, 39(4), 633–651. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pickering, M. J., & Gambi, C.
(2018) Predicting while comprehending language: A theory and review. Psychological Bulletin, 144(10), 1002–1044. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pickering, M. J., & Garrod, S.
(2007) Do people use language production to make predictions during comprehension? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(3), 105–110. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rabagliati, H., Gambi, C., & Pickering, M. J.
(2016) Learning to predict or predicting to learn? Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 31(1), 94–105. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Reitter, D., Keller, F., & Moore, J. D.
(2011) A computational cognitive model of syntactic priming. Cognitive Science, 35(4), 587–637. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rissman, L., Legendre, G., & Landau, B.
(2013) Abstract morphosyntax in two- and three-year-old children: Evidence from priming. Language Learning and Development, 9(3), 278–292. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rowland, C. F., Chang, F., Ambridge, B., Pine, J. M., & Lieven, E. V. M.
(2012) The development of abstract syntax: Evidence from structural priming and the lexical boost. Cognition, 125(1), 49–63. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Savage, C., Lieven, E., Theakston, A., & Tomasello, M.
(2003) Testing the abstractness of children’s linguistic representations: lexical and structural priming of syntactic constructions in young children. Developmental Science, 6(5), 557–567. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2006) Structural priming as implicit learning in language acquisition: The persistence of lexical and structural priming in 4-year-olds. Language Learning and Development, 2(1), 27–49. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Serratrice, L., Hesketh, A., & Ashworth, R.
(2015) The use of reported speech in children’s narratives: A priming study. First Language, 35(1), 68–87. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shimpi, P. M., Gámez, P. B., Huttenlocher, J., & Vasilyeva, M.
(2007) Syntactic priming in 3- and 4-year-old children: Evidence for abstract representations of transitive and dative forms. Developmental Psychology, 43(6), 1334–1346. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Skarabela, B., & Serratrice, L.
(2009) ‘The doctor’s mother’ or ‘the mother of the doctor’? Syntactic priming of possessive noun phrases in English preschoolers. In Online supplement of the proceedings of the Boston University Conference on Language Development (Vol. 33), Boston University, Boston, MA, 31 October– 2 November 2008.Google Scholar
Thothathiri, M., & Snedeker, J.
(2008) Syntactic priming during language comprehension in three- and four-year-old children. Journal of Memory and Language, 58(2), 188–213. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Turner, E. A., & Rommetveit, R.
(1967) The acquisition of sentence voice and reversibility. Child Development, 38(3), 649–660. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vasilyeva, M., Huttenlocher, J., & Waterfall, H.
(2006) Effects of language intervention on syntactic skill levels in preschoolers. Developmental Psychology, 42(1), 164–174. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vasilyeva, M., & Waterfall, H.
(2012) Beyond syntactic priming: Evidence for activation of alternative syntactic structures. Journal of Child Language, 39(2), 258–283. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vasilyeva, M., Waterfall, H., Gámez, P. B., Gómez, L. E., Bowers, E., & Shimpi, P. M.
(2010) Cross-linguistic syntactic priming in bilingual children. Journal of Child Language, 37(5), 1047–1064. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Weisleder, A., & Fernald, A.
(2013) Talking to children matters: Early language experience strengthens processing and builds vocabulary. Psychological Science, 24(11), 2143–2152. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Whitehurst, G. J., & Novak, G.
(1973) Modeling, imitation training, and the acquisition of sentence phrases. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 16(2), 332–345. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zimmerman, F. J., Gilkerson, J., Richards, J. A., Christakis, D. A., Xu, D., Gray, S., & Yapanel, U.
(2009) Teaching by listening: The importance of adult-child conversations to language development. Pediatrics, 124(1), 342–349. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 1 other publications

Buckle, Leone, Holly P. Branigan, Laura Lindsay & Katherine Messenger
2024. Now You See Me, Now You don’t: Children Learn Grammatical Choices During Online Socially Contingent Video and Audio Interactions. Language Learning and Development  pp. 1 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 28 march 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.