References
Bencini, G., & Valian, V.
(2008) Abstract sentence representations in 3-year-olds: Evidence from language production and comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 97–113. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bernolet, S., Collina, S., & Hartsuiker, R.
(2016) The persistence of syntactic priming revisited. Journal of Memory and Language, 91, 99–116. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bock, J. K.
(1986) Syntactic persistence in language production. Cognitive Psychology, 18, 355–387. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bock, J. K., Dell, G., Chang, F., & Onishi, K.
(2007) Persistent structural priming from language comprehension to language production. Cognition, 104, 437–458. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bock, J. K., & Griffin, Z.
(2000) The persistence of structural priming: Transient activation or implicit learning? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129, 177–192. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bock, J. K., & Loebell, H.
(1990) Framing sentences. Cognition, 35, 1–39. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Branigan, H., & McLean, J.
(2016) What children learn from adults’ utterances: An ephemeral lexical boost and persistent syntactic priming in adult-child dialogue. Journal of Memory and Language, 91, 141–157. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Branigan, H., McLean, J., & Jones, M.
(2005) A blue cat or a cat that is blue? Evidence for abstract syntax in young children’s noun phrases. In A. Brugos, M. Clark-Cotton, & S. Ha (Eds.), BUCLD29: Proceedings of the 29th annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 109–121). Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Branigan, H., Pickering, M., & Cleland, A.
(2000) Syntactic co-ordination in dialogue. Cognition, 75, B13–B25. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Branigan, H., Pickering, M., McLean, J., & Cleland, A.
(2007) Syntactic alignment and participant role in dialogue. Cognition, 104, 163–197. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Branigan, H., Pickering, M., McLean, J., & Stewart, A.
(2006) The role of local and global syntactic structure in language production: Evidence from syntactic priming. Language and Cognitive Processes, 21, 974–1010. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chang, F., Bock, K., & Goldberg, A.
(2003) Can thematic roles leave traces of their places? Cognition, 90, 29–49. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fehér, O., Wonnacott, E., & Smith, K.
(2016) Structural priming in artificial languages and the regularisation of unpredictable variation. Journal of Memory and Language, 91, 158–180. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ferreira, V., & Bock, J. K.
(2006) The functions of structural priming. Language and Cognitive Processes, 21, 1011–1029. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ferreira, V., Bock, J. K., Wilson, M., & Cohen, N.
(2008) Memory for syntax despite amnesia. Psychological Science, 19, 940–946. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Finestack, L.
(2018) Evaluation of an explicit intervention to teach novel grammatical forms to children with developmental language disorder. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 61, 2062–2075. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gámez, P., & Shimpi, P.
(2016) Structural priming in Spanish as evidence of implicit learning. Journal of Child Language, 43, 207–233. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Garraffa, M., Coco, M., & Branigan, H.
(2015) Effects of immediate and cumulative syntactic experience in language impairment: Evidence of priming of subject relatives in children with SLI. Language Learning and Development, 11, 18–40. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gómez, R.
(2002) Variability and detection of invariant structure. Psychological Science, 13, 431–436. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gruberg, N., Ostrand, R., Momma, S., & Ferreira, V.
(2019) Syntactic entrainment: The repetition of syntactic structures in event descriptions. Journal of Memory and Language, 107, 216–232. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hartsuiker, R., Bernolet, S., Schoonbaert, S., Speybroeck, S., & Vanderelst, D.
(2008) Syntactic priming persists while the lexical boost decays: Evidence from written and spoken dialogue. Journal of Memory and Language, 58, 214–238. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hsu, D.-B.
(2014) Structural priming as learning: Evidence from Mandarin-Learning 5-year-olds. Language Acquisition, 21, 156–172. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Huttenlocher, J., Vailyeva, M., & Shimpi, P.
(2004) Syntactic priming in young children. Journal of Memory and Language, 50, 182–195. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Krok, W., & Leonard, L.
(2018) Verb variability and morphosyntactic priming with typically developing 2- and 3-year-olds. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 61, 2996–3009. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Leonard, L.
(2014) Children with specific language impairment (2nd ed.). The MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Leonard, L., & Deevy, P.
(2011) Input distribution influences degree of auxiliary use by children with SLI. Cognitive Linguistics, 22, 247–273. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Leonard, L., Fey, M., Deevy, P., & Bredin-Oja, S.
(2015) Input sources of third person singular –s inconsistency in children with and without specific language impairment. Journal of Child Language, 42, 786–820. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Leonard, L., Miller, C., Deevy, P., Rauf, L., Gerber, E., & Charest, M.
(2002) Production operations and the use of nonfinite verbs by children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 45, 744–758. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Leonard, L., Miller, C., Grela, B., Holland, A., Gerber, E., & Petucci, M.
(2000) Production operations contribute to the grammatical morpheme limitations of children with specific language impairment. Journal of Memory and Language, 43, 362–378. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McCauley, R., Fey, M., & Gillam, R.
(2017) Treatment of language disorders in children (2nd ed.). Paul H. Brookes.Google Scholar
Messenger, K., Branigan, H., & McLean, J.
(2011) Evidence for (shared) abstract structure underlying children’s short and full passives. Cognition, 121, 268–274. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Messenger, K., Branigan, H., McLean, J., & Sorace, A.
(2012) Is young children’s passive syntax semantically constrained? Evidence from syntactic priming. Journal of Memory and Language, 66, 568–587. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Miller, C., & Deevy, P.
(2006) Structural priming in children with and without specific language impairment. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 20, 387–399. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Olswang, L., Bain, B., & Johnson, G.
(1992) Using dynamic assessment with children with language disorders. In S. Warren & J. Reichle (Eds.), Causes and effects in communication and language intervention (Vol. 1, pp. 187–216). Paul H. Brookes.Google Scholar
Pelham, S.
(2011) The input ambiguity hypothesis and case blindness: An account of cross-linguistic and intra-linguistic differences in case errors. Journal of Child Language, 38, 235–272. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pickering, M., & Branigan, H.
(1998) The representation of verbs: Evidence from syntactic priming in language production. Journal of Memory and Language, 39, 633–651. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Plante, E., Ogilvie, T., Vance, R., Aguilar, J., Dailey, N., Meyers, C., … Burton, R.
(2014) Variability in the language input to children enhances learning in a treatment context. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 23, 530–545. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rowland, C., Chang, F., Ambridge, B., Pine, J., & Lieven, E.
(2012) The development of abstract syntax: Evidence from structural priming and the lexical boost. Cognition, 125, 49–63. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Savage, C., Lieven, E., Theakston, A., & Tomasello, M.
(2003) Testing the abstractness of children’s linguistic representations: Lexical and structural priming of syntactic constructions in young children. Developmental Science, 6, 557–567. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2006) Structural priming as implicit learning in language acquisition: The persistence of lexical and structural priming in 4-year-olds. Language Learning and Development, 2, 27–49. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Scheepers, C., Raffray, C., & Myachykov, A.
(2017) The lexical boost is not diagnostic of lexically-specific syntactic representations. Journal of Memory and Language, 95, 102–115. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Theakston, A., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M.
(2003) The role of input in the acquisition of third person singular verbs in English. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 46, 863–877. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Thothathiri, A., & Snedeker, J.
(2008) Give and take: Syntactic priming during spoken language comprehension. Cognition, 108, 51–68. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vasilyeva, M., Huttenlocher, J. & Waterfall, H.
(2006) Effects of language intervention on syntactic skill levels in preschoolers. Developmental Psychology, 42, 164–174. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Weber, K., Christiansen, M., Indefrey, P., & Hagoort, P.
(2019) Primed from the start: Syntactic priming during the first days of language learning. Language Learning, 69, 198–221. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wisman Weil, L., & Leonard, L.
(2017) Case assignment in English-speaking children: A paired priming paradigm. Journal of Child Language, 44, 943–967. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 1 other publications

Buckle, Leone, Holly P. Branigan, Laura Lindsay & Katherine Messenger
2024. Now You See Me, Now You don’t: Children Learn Grammatical Choices During Online Socially Contingent Video and Audio Interactions. Language Learning and Development  pp. 1 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 28 march 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.