References
Bencini, G., & Valian, V.
(2008) Abstract sentence representations in 3-year-olds: Evidence from language production and comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 59 , 97–113. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bernolet, S., Collina, S., & Hartsuiker, R.
(2016) The persistence of syntactic priming revisited. Journal of Memory and Language, 91 , 99–116. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bock, J. K.
(1986) Syntactic persistence in language production. Cognitive Psychology, 18 , 355–387. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bock, J. K., Dell, G., Chang, F., & Onishi, K.
(2007) Persistent structural priming from language comprehension to language production. Cognition, 104 , 437–458. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bock, J. K., & Griffin, Z.
(2000) The persistence of structural priming: Transient activation or implicit learning? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129 , 177–192. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bock, J. K., & Loebell, H.
(1990) Framing sentences. Cognition, 35 , 1–39. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Branigan, H., & McLean, J.
(2016) What children learn from adults’ utterances: An ephemeral lexical boost and persistent syntactic priming in adult-child dialogue. Journal of Memory and Language, 91 , 141–157. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Branigan, H., McLean, J., & Jones, M.
(2005) A blue cat or a cat that is blue? Evidence for abstract syntax in young children’s noun phrases. In A. Brugos, M. Clark-Cotton, & S. Ha (Eds.), BUCLD29: Proceedings of the 29th annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 109–121). Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Branigan, H., Pickering, M., & Cleland, A.
(2000) Syntactic co-ordination in dialogue. Cognition, 75 , B13–B25. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Branigan, H., Pickering, M., McLean, J., & Cleland, A.
(2007) Syntactic alignment and participant role in dialogue. Cognition, 104 , 163–197. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Branigan, H., Pickering, M., McLean, J., & Stewart, A.
(2006) The role of local and global syntactic structure in language production: Evidence from syntactic priming. Language and Cognitive Processes, 21 , 974–1010. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Chang, F., Bock, K., & Goldberg, A.
(2003) Can thematic roles leave traces of their places? Cognition, 90 , 29–49. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fehér, O., Wonnacott, E., & Smith, K.
(2016) Structural priming in artificial languages and the regularisation of unpredictable variation. Journal of Memory and Language, 91 , 158–180. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ferreira, V., & Bock, J. K.
(2006) The functions of structural priming. Language and Cognitive Processes, 21 , 1011–1029. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ferreira, V., Bock, J. K., Wilson, M., & Cohen, N.
(2008) Memory for syntax despite amnesia. Psychological Science, 19 , 940–946. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Finestack, L.
(2018) Evaluation of an explicit intervention to teach novel grammatical forms to children with developmental language disorder. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 61 , 2062–2075. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gámez, P., & Shimpi, P.
(2016) Structural priming in Spanish as evidence of implicit learning. Journal of Child Language, 43 , 207–233. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Garraffa, M., Coco, M., & Branigan, H.
(2015) Effects of immediate and cumulative syntactic experience in language impairment: Evidence of priming of subject relatives in children with SLI. Language Learning and Development, 11 , 18–40. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gómez, R.
(2002) Variability and detection of invariant structure. Psychological Science, 13 , 431–436. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gruberg, N., Ostrand, R., Momma, S., & Ferreira, V.
(2019) Syntactic entrainment: The repetition of syntactic structures in event descriptions. Journal of Memory and Language, 107 , 216–232. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hartsuiker, R., Bernolet, S., Schoonbaert, S., Speybroeck, S., & Vanderelst, D.
(2008) Syntactic priming persists while the lexical boost decays: Evidence from written and spoken dialogue. Journal of Memory and Language, 58 , 214–238. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hsu, D.-B.
(2014) Structural priming as learning: Evidence from Mandarin-Learning 5-year-olds. Language Acquisition, 21 , 156–172. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Huttenlocher, J., Vailyeva, M., & Shimpi, P.
(2004) Syntactic priming in young children. Journal of Memory and Language, 50 , 182–195. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Krok, W., & Leonard, L.
(2018) Verb variability and morphosyntactic priming with typically developing 2- and 3-year-olds. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 61 , 2996–3009. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Leonard, L.
(2014) Children with specific language impairment (2nd ed.). The MIT Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Leonard, L., & Deevy, P.
(2011) Input distribution influences degree of auxiliary use by children with SLI. Cognitive Linguistics, 22 , 247–273. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Leonard, L., Fey, M., Deevy, P., & Bredin-Oja, S.
(2015) Input sources of third person singular –s inconsistency in children with and without specific language impairment. Journal of Child Language, 42 , 786–820. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Leonard, L., Miller, C., Deevy, P., Rauf, L., Gerber, E., & Charest, M.
(2002) Production operations and the use of nonfinite verbs by children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 45 , 744–758. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Leonard, L., Miller, C., Grela, B., Holland, A., Gerber, E., & Petucci, M.
(2000) Production operations contribute to the grammatical morpheme limitations of children with specific language impairment. Journal of Memory and Language, 43 , 362–378. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
McCauley, R., Fey, M., & Gillam, R.
(2017) Treatment of language disorders in children (2nd ed.). Paul H. Brookes.Google Scholar
Messenger, K., Branigan, H., & McLean, J.
(2011) Evidence for (shared) abstract structure underlying children’s short and full passives. Cognition, 121 , 268–274. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Messenger, K., Branigan, H., McLean, J., & Sorace, A.
(2012) Is young children’s passive syntax semantically constrained? Evidence from syntactic priming. Journal of Memory and Language, 66 , 568–587. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Miller, C., & Deevy, P.
(2006) Structural priming in children with and without specific language impairment. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 20 , 387–399. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Olswang, L., Bain, B., & Johnson, G.
(1992) Using dynamic assessment with children with language disorders. In S. Warren & J. Reichle (Eds.), Causes and effects in communication and language intervention (Vol. 1, pp. 187–216). Paul H. Brookes.Google Scholar
Pelham, S.
(2011) The input ambiguity hypothesis and case blindness: An account of cross-linguistic and intra-linguistic differences in case errors. Journal of Child Language, 38 , 235–272. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pickering, M., & Branigan, H.
(1998) The representation of verbs: Evidence from syntactic priming in language production. Journal of Memory and Language, 39 , 633–651. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Plante, E., Ogilvie, T., Vance, R., Aguilar, J., Dailey, N., Meyers, C., … Burton, R.
(2014) Variability in the language input to children enhances learning in a treatment context. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 23 , 530–545. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rowland, C., Chang, F., Ambridge, B., Pine, J., & Lieven, E.
(2012) The development of abstract syntax: Evidence from structural priming and the lexical boost. Cognition, 125 , 49–63. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Savage, C., Lieven, E., Theakston, A., & Tomasello, M.
(2003) Testing the abstractness of children’s linguistic representations: Lexical and structural priming of syntactic constructions in young children. Developmental Science, 6 , 557–567. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2006) Structural priming as implicit learning in language acquisition: The persistence of lexical and structural priming in 4-year-olds. Language Learning and Development, 2 , 27–49. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Scheepers, C., Raffray, C., & Myachykov, A.
(2017) The lexical boost is not diagnostic of lexically-specific syntactic representations. Journal of Memory and Language, 95 , 102–115. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Theakston, A., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M.
(2003) The role of input in the acquisition of third person singular verbs in English. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 46 , 863–877. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Thothathiri, A., & Snedeker, J.
(2008) Give and take: Syntactic priming during spoken language comprehension. Cognition, 108 , 51–68. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Vasilyeva, M., Huttenlocher, J. & Waterfall, H.
(2006) Effects of language intervention on syntactic skill levels in preschoolers. Developmental Psychology, 42 , 164–174. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Weber, K., Christiansen, M., Indefrey, P., & Hagoort, P.
(2019) Primed from the start: Syntactic priming during the first days of language learning. Language Learning, 69 , 198–221. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wisman Weil, L., & Leonard, L.
(2017) Case assignment in English-speaking children: A paired priming paradigm. Journal of Child Language, 44 , 943–967. CrossrefGoogle Scholar