Article published In:
Written Language & Literacy
Vol. 19:1 (2016) ► pp.114130
References
Abu-Rabia, S
(2003) The influence of working memory on reading and creative writing processes in a second language. Educational Psychology 231: 209–222. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Anderson, N. J
(2007) Active skills for reading. Toronto: Thomson.Google Scholar
Aronson, Trudy
(2006) English grammar digest. New York: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Au, M. J, G. Buschkuehl, J. Duncan & S. M. Jaeggi
(2016) There is no convincing evidence that working memory training is NOT effective: A reply to Melby-Lervåg and Hulme (2015). Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 23(1): 331–337. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Au, J., E. Sheehan, N. Tsai, G. J. Duncan, M. Buschkuehl & S. M. Jaeggi
(2015) Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory: A meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 22(2): 366–377. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A. D
(2001) Is working memory still working? American Psychologist 561: 849–864. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cattell, R
(1949) Culture free intelligence test, scale 1, handbook. Champaign, IL: Institute of Personality and Ability Testing.Google Scholar
Cattell R. B
(1987) Intelligence: Its structure, growth and action. Amsterdam, North-Holland: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Chooi, W. T. & L. A. Thompson
(2012) Working memory training does not improve intelligence in healthy young adults. Intelligence 401: 531. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dafouz-Milne, Emma
(2008) The pragmatic role of textual and interpersonal meta-discourse markers in the construction and attainment of persuasion: A cross-linguistic study of newspaper discourse. Journal of Pragmatics, 40 (1), 95-113. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Falahati, R
(2010) The relationship between students’ IQ and their ability to use transitional words and expressions in writing. In R. Waldie, M. Teshigawa, A. Bedenbender & X. Wu (eds.), Proceedings of the 19th north west linguistics conference, 11–19. University of Toronto.Google Scholar
Farvardin, M. T., A. Afghari & M. Kusha
(2014) The effect of dual n-back task training on phonological memory expansion in adult EFL learners at the beginner level. English Language Teaching Journal 9(7): 137–143. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Harrison, T. L., Z. Shipstead, K. L. Hicks, D. Z. Hambrick, T. S. Redick & R. W. Engle
(2013) Working memory training may increase working memory capacity but not fluid intelligence. Psychological Science 24(12): 2409–2419. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Horn, J. L. & R. B. Cattell
(1966) Refinement and test of the theory of fluid and crystallizedgeneral intelligences. Journal of Educational Psychology 571: 258–270. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jaeggi, S. M., M. Buschkuehl, J. Jonides & W. J. Perrig
(2008) Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 1051: 6829–6833. DOI logo
Jensen, A. R
(1998) The G factor: The science of mental ability. Westport: Praeger.Google Scholar
Kane, M. J. & R. W. Engle
(2002) The role of prefrontal cortex in working-memory capacity, executive attention, and general fluid intelligence: An individual-differences perspective. Psychology Bulletin Review 91: 637–671. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kellogg, R. T
(1996) A model of working memory in writing. In C. M. Levy & S. E. Ransdell (eds.), The science of writing, 57–72. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
(2001) Competition for working memory among writing processes. American Journal of Psychology 114(2): 175–192. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Khodadadi, M., A. Mashhadi & H. Amani
(2014) N-back software computer program. Tehran: Institute of Cognitive Behavioral Research.Google Scholar
Kunda, M., K. McGreggor & A. K. Goel
(2013) A computational model for solving problems from the RPM intelligence test using iconic visual representations. Cognitive Systems Research 2(23): 47–66. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kundu, B., D. W. Sutterer, S. M. Emrich & B. R. Postle
(2013) Strengthened effective connectivity underlies transfer of working memory training to tests of short-term memory and attention. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience 331: 8705–15. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kyllonen, P. C & R. E. Christal
(1990) Reasoning ability is little more than working memory capacity?! Intelligence 741: 389–433. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lee, L. & E. Gunderson
(2002) Select readings. Oxford: OUP.Google Scholar
Levy, C. M. & S. Ransdell
(2001) Writing with concurrent memory loads. In T. Olive & C. M. Levy (eds.), Contemporary tools and techniques for studying writing, 9–30. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.Google Scholar
Logie, R. H
(1995) Visual-spatial working memory. Sussex: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Lohman, F
(2001) Fluid intelligence, inductive reasoning, and working memory: Where the theory of Multiple Intelligences falls short. Paper presented at the Annual meeting of the American educational research association in Seattle , WA.
Marandi, S
(2002) Contrastive EAP rhetoric: Meta-discourse in Persian vs. English. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Tehran.Google Scholar
Olive, T
(2003) Working memory in writing: Empirical evidence from the dual task technique. European Psychologist 91: 32–43. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Raven, J
(2000) The Raven's progressive matrices: Change and stability over culture and time. Cognitive Psychology 411: 1–48. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Redick, T. S., Z. Shipstead, T. L. Harrison, K. L. Hicks, D. E. Fried, D. Z. Hambrick & R. W. Engle
(2013) No evidence of intelligence improvement after working memory training: A randomized, placebo-controlled study. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 142(2): 359–379. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schneider, W. J. & K. S. McGrew
(2012) The Cattell-Horn-Carroll model of intelligence. In D. P. Flanagan & P. L. Harrison (eds.), Contemporary intellectual assessment: Theories, tests, and issues, 99–144. New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Snow, R. E
(1994) Abilities in academic tasks. In R. J. Sternberg & R. K. Wagner (eds.), Mind in context: Interactionist perspectives on human intelligence, 3–37. Cambridge, England: CUP.Google Scholar
Stephenson, L. C
(2010) Does training to increase working memory capacity improve fluid intelligence? Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Claremont, California.Google Scholar
Vigneau, F. & D. A. Bors
(2008) The quest for item types based on information processing: An analysis of Raven's advanced progressive matrices with a consideration of gender differences. Intelligence 5(5), 702–710. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wilkinson, A
(1989) Assessing language development: The crediting project. In Aviva Freedman, Ian Pringle & Janice Yalden (eds.), Learning to write first language/second Language, 33-45. New York: Longman.Google Scholar