Part of
The Acquisition of the Present
Edited by Dalila Ayoun
[Not in series 196] 2015
► pp. 120
References (36)
References
Ambridge, B., Kidd, E.J., Rowland, C.F. & Theakston, A.L. 2015. The ubiquity of frequency effects in first language. Journal of Child Language 42: 239–273. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Andersen, R.W. 1986. The need for native language comparison data in interpreting second language data. Forum Lecture, 1986 TESOL Summer Institute, University of Hawai'i at Ma'noa.
. 1990. Models, processes, principles, and strategies: Second language acquisition in and out of the classroom. In Second Language Acquisition – Foreign Language Learning, B. VanPatten & J.F. Lee (eds), 45–78. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Andersen, R. & Shirai, Y. 1994. Discourse motivations for some cognitive acquisition principles. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 16: 133–156. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Andersen, R.W. & Shirai, Y. 1996. Primacy of aspect in first and second language acquisition. In Handbook of Second Language Acquisition, W.C. Ritchie & T.K. Bhatia (eds), 527–570. San Diego CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Antinucci, F. & Miller, R. 1976. How children talk about what happened. Journal of Child Language 3: 167–189. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Berman, R.A. & Dromi, E. 1984. On marking time without aspect in child language. Papers & Reports on Child Language Development 23: 23–32.Google Scholar
Behrens, H. 1993. Temporal Reference in German Child Language: Form and Function of Early Verb Use. PhD dissertation, University of Amsterdam.
Bloom, L., Lifter, K. & Hafitz, J. 1980. Semantics of verbs and the development of verb inflection in child language. Language 56: 386–412. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bloom, L., Merkin, W. & Wootten, J. 1982. Wh-questions: Linguistic factors that contribute to the sequence of acquisition. Child Development 53: 1084–1092.Google Scholar
Bowerman, M. 1976. Semantic factors in the acquisition of rules for word use and sentence construction. In Directions in Normal and Deficient Child Language, D. Morehead & A. Morehead (eds), 191–209. Baltimore MD: University Park Press.Google Scholar
Brown, R. 1973. A First Language: The Early Stages. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Comrie, B. 1976. Aspect. Cambridge: CUP.Google Scholar
. 1985. Tense. Cambridge: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dowty, D. 1979. Word Meaning and Montague Grammar. Dordrecht: Reidel. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Forner, M. 1979. The mother as LAD: Interaction between order and frequency of parental input and child production. In Studies in First and Second Language Acquisition, F.R. Eckman & A.J. Hastings (eds), 17–44. Rowley MA: Newbury House.Google Scholar
Hatch, E.M. 1974. Second language learning - universals? Working Papers on Bilingualism 3: 1–17.Google Scholar
Johnson, V., de Villiers, J. & Seymour, H. 2005. Agreement without understanding? The case of third person singular /s/. First Language 25: 317–330. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Li, P. & Shirai, Y. 2000. The Acquisition of Lexical and Grammatical Aspect. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
MacWhinney, B. 2000. The CHILDES Project: Tools for Analyzing Talk, 3rd edn. Mahwah NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. & Snow, C. E. 1985. The child language data exchange system. Journal of Child Language 12: 271–296. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
MacWhinney, B. & Snow, C.E. 1990. The child language data exchange system: An update. Journal of Child Language 17: 457–472. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Moerk, E. 1980. Relationships between parental input frequencies and children’s language acquisition: A reanalysis of Brown’s data. Journal of Child Language 7: 105–118. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sachs, J. 1983. Talking about the there and then: The emergence of displaced reference in parent-child discourse. In Children's Language, K.E. Nelson (ed.), 1–28. Hillsdale NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Shirai, Y. 1991. Primacy of Aspect in Language Acquisition: Simplified Input and Prototype. PhD dissertation, University of California, Los Angeles.
. 1994. On the overgeneralization of progressive marking on stative verbs: Bioprogram or input? First language 14: 67–82. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. 1998. The emergence of tense/aspect morphology in Japanese: Universal predisposition? First Language 18: 281–310. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shirai, Y. & Andersen, R.W. 1995. The acquisition of tense-aspect morphology: A prototype account. Language 71: 743–762. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Smith, C. 1991. The Parameter of Aspect, 1st edn. Dordrecht: Kluwer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. 1997. The Parameter of Aspect, 2nd edn. Dordrecht: Kluwer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stoll, S. 1998. The role of Aktionsart in the acquisition of Russian aspect. First language 18: 351–377. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Theakston, A., Lieven, E. & Tomasello, M. 2003. The role of the input in the acquisition of third person singular verbs in English. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 46: 863–877. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tomassello, M. 1992. First Verbs: A Case Study of Early Grammatical Development. Cambridge: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vendler, Z. 1957. Verbs and times. Philosophical Review 66: 143–160. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Weist, R.M. 2002. The first language acquisition of tense and aspect: a review. In Tense- Aspect Morphology in L2 Acquisition [language Acquisition and Language Disorders 27], M.R. Salaberry & Y. Shirai (Eds), 21–78. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Weist, R.M., Wysocka, H., Witkowska-Stadnik, K., Buczowska, E. & Konieczna, E. 1984. The defective tense hypothesis: On the emergence of tense and aspect in child Polish. Journal of Child Language 11: 347–374. DOI logoGoogle Scholar