Part of
Crossroads Semantics: Computation, experiment and grammar
Edited by Hilke Reckman, Lisa Lai-Shen Cheng, Maarten Hijzelendoorn and Rint Sybesma
[Not in series 210] 2017
► pp. 2337
References

References

Bargh, John A., Mark Chen & Lara Burrows
1996Automaticity of social behavior: Direct effects of trait construct and stereotype activation on action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 71(2). 230–244. DOI logo.Google Scholar
Bornmann, Lutz, Rüdiger Mutz & Hans-Dieter Daniel
2010A reliability-generalization study of journal peer reviews: a multilevel meta-analysis of inter-rater reliability and its determinants. PLoS ONE 5(12). e14331. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Breiman, Leo
2001Statistical Modeling: The Two Cultures (with comments and a rejoinder by the author). Statistical Science 16(3). 199–231. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Button, Katherine S., John P. A. Ioannidis, Claire Mokrysz, Brian A. Nosek, Jonathan Flint, Emma S. J. Robinson & Marcus R. Munafò
2013Power failure: why small sample size undermines the reliability of neuroscience. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 14(5). 365–376. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Carp, Joshua
2012The secret lives of experiments: Methods reporting in the fMRI literature. NeuroImage 63(1). 289–300. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cohen, Jacob
1994The earth is round (p < .05). American Psychologist 49(12). 997–1003. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Collobert, Ronan & Jason Weston
2008A unified architecture for natural language processing: deep neural networks with multitask learning. the 25th international conference. New York, NY: ACM. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Coyne, James C & Jacob N de Voogd
2012Are we witnessing the decline effect in the Type D personality literature? What can be learned? Journal of psychosomatic research 73(6). 401–407. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Doyen, Stéphane, Olivier Klein, Cora-Lise Pichon & Axel Cleeremans
2012Behavioral priming: It’s all in the mind, but whose mind? PLoS ONE 7(1). e29081. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dreber, Anna, Thomas Pfeiffer, Johan Almenberg, Siri Isaksson, Brad Wilson, Yiling Chen, Brian A. Nosek & Magnus Johannesson
2015Using prediction markets to estimate the reproducibility of scientific research. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 112(50). 15343–15347. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Falk, Ruma & Charles W. Greenbaum
1995Significance tests die hard: The amazing persistence of a probabilistic misconception. Theory & Psychology 5(1). 75–98. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fanelli, Daniele
2010“Positive” results increase down the hierarchy of the sciences. PLoS ONE 5(4). e10068. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fugelsang, Jonathan A., Courtney B. Stein, Adam E. Green & Kevin N. Dunbar
2004Theory and data interactions of the scientific mind: Evidence from the molecular and the cognitive laboratory. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie expérimentale 58(2). 86–95. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gigerenzer, Gerd
2004Mindless statistics. The Journal of Socio-Economics 33(5). 587–606. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gilbert, Daniel T., Gary King, Stephen Pettigrew & Timothy D. Wilson
2016Comment on “Estimating the reproducibility of psychological science”. Science 351(6277). 1037–1037. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Greenberg, Steven A.
2009How citation distortions create unfounded authority: analysis of a citation network. BMJ (Clinical research ed.) 339. b2680. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Greenwald, Anthony G.
2012There is nothing so theoretical as a good method. Perspectives on Psychological Science. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hill, Ray
2004Multiple sudden infant deaths – coincidence or beyond coincidence? Paediatric and perinatal epidemiology 18(5). 320–326. Blackwell Science Ltd. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ioannidis, John P. A.
2005Why most published research findings are false. PLoS Medicine 2(8). e124. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ioannidis, John P A.
2008Why most discovered true associations are inflated. Epidemiology 19(5). 640–648. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ioannidis, John P.A.
2012Why science is not necessarily self-correcting. Perspectives on Psychological Science: A Journal of the Association for Psychological Science 7(6). 645–654. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ioannidis, John P. A., Kevin W. Boyack, Henry Small, Aaron A. Sorensen & Richard Klavans
2014Bibliometrics: Is your most cited work your best? Nature 514(7524). 561–562. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kahneman, Daniel
2013Thinking, fast and slow. Farrar, Straus and GirouxGoogle Scholar
Klein, Richard A., Kate A. Ratliff, Michelangelo Vianello, Reginald B. Adams, Štěpán Bahník, Michael J. Bernstein, Konrad Bocian, et al.
2014Investigating variation in replicability. Social Psychology 45(3). 142–152. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kramer, Adam D. I., Jamie E. Guillory & Jeffrey T. Hancock
2014Experimental evidence of massive-scale emotional contagion through social networks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 111(24). 8788–8790. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kriegeskorte, Nikolaus
2012An emerging consensus for open evaluation: 18 visions for the future of scientific publishing. 1–5. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kriegeskorte, Nikolaus, Marieke Mur & Peter Bandettini
2008Representational similarity analysis – connecting the branches of systems neuroscience. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience 2Google Scholar
Lazer, David, Ryan Kennedy, Gary King & Alessandro Vespignani
2014Big data. The parable of Google Flu: Traps in big data analysis. Science 343(6176). 1203–1205. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lindley, Dennis V.
2000The philosophy of statistics. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series D (The Statistician) 49(3). 293–337Google Scholar
Loftus, Geoffrey R.
1993A picture is worth a thousand p values: On the irrelevance of hypothesis testing in the microcomputer age. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers 25(2). 250–256. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Maxwell, Scott E.
2004The persistence of underpowered studies in psychological research: Causes, consequences, and remedies. Psychological Methods 9(2). 147–163. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Meehl, Paul E.
1978Theoretical risks and tabular asterisks: Sir Karl, Sir Ronald, and the slow progress of soft psychology. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
1993Philosophy of science: Help or hindrance? Psychological Reports 72(3). 707–733. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Meehl, PE.
1967Theory-testing in psychology and physics: A methodological paradox. Philosophy of science 34(2). 103–115. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Miller, G.
2012The smartphone psychology manifesto. Perspectives on Psychological Science: A Journal of the Association for Psychological Science 7(3). 221–237. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Munafo, Marcus
2013Deep impact: Unintended consequences of journal rank. 1–33.Google Scholar
Nickerson, R. S.
2000Null hypothesis significance testing: A review of an old and continuing controversy. Psychological methods 5(2). 241–301. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nuijten, Michèle B., Chris H. J. Hartgerink, Marcel A. L. M. van Assen, Sacha Epskamp & Jelte M. Wicherts
2015The prevalence of statistical reporting errors in psychology (1985–2013). Behavior Research Methods. 1–22. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Open Science Collaboration
2015Estimating the reproducibility of psychological science. Science 349(6251). aac4716–aac4716. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pfeiffer, Thomas, Lars Bertram & John P. A. Ioannidis
2011Quantifying selective reporting and the Proteus phenomenon for multiple datasets with similar bias. PLoS ONE 6(3). e18362. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rodriguez-Esteban, Raul, Ivan Iossifov & Andrey Rzhetsky
2006Imitating manual curation of text-mined facts in biomedicine. PLoS computational biology 2(9). e118. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rosenthal, Robert
1979The file drawer problem and tolerance for null results. Psychological Bulletin 86(3). 638–641. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rosenthal, Robert & John Gaito
1963The interpretation of levels of significance by psychological researchers. The Journal of Psychology 55(1). 33–38. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rutledge, Robb B., Nikolina Skandali, Peter Dayan & Raymond J. Dolan
2014A computational and neural model of momentary subjective well-being. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 111(33). 12252–12257. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shmueli, Galit
2010To explain or to predict? Statistical Science 25(3). 289–310. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Simmons, Joseph P., Leif D. Nelson & Uri Simonsohn
2011False-positive psychology: Undisclosed flexibility in data collection and analysis allows presenting anything as significant. Psychological Science 22(11). 1359–1366. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Simonsohn, Uri, Leif D. Nelson & Joseph P. Simmons
2014P-curve: A key to the file-drawer. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 143(2). 534. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Thompson, William C. & Edward L. Schumann
1987Interpretation of statistical evidence in criminal trials: The prosecutor’s fallacy and the defense attorney’s fallacy. Law and Human Behavior 11(3). Springer. 167–187. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tversky, Amos & Daniel Kahneman
1971Belief in the law of small numbers. Psychological Bulletin 76(2). 105. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tyler, Lorraine K, Teresa P. L. Cheung, Barry J. Devereux & Alex Clarke
2013Syntactic computations in the language network: Characterizing dynamic network properties using representational similarity analysis. Frontiers in Psychology 4. 271. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wainer, Howard
2007The most dangerous equation. American Scientist. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wicherts, Jelte M. & Marcel A. L. M. van Assen
2012Research fraud: Speed up reviews of misconduct. Nature 488(7413). 591–591. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yarkoni, Tal, Russell A. Poldrack, Thomas E. Nichols, David C. Van Essen & Tor D. Wager
2011Large-scale automated synthesis of human functional neuroimaging data. Nature methods 8(8). 665–670. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yong, Ed.
2012Replication studies: Bad copy. Nature. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Young, Neal S., John P. A. Ioannidis & Omar Al-Ubaydli
2008Why current publication practices may distort science. PLoS Medicine 5(10). e201. DOI logoGoogle Scholar