Part of
Atypical Language Development in Romance Languages
Edited by Eva Aguilar-Mediavilla, Lucía Buil-Legaz, Raúl López-Penadés, Victor A. Sanchez-Azanza and Daniel Adrover-Roig
[Not in series 223] 2019
► pp. 3756
References
Aarnoudse-Moens, C. S. H., Smidts, D. P., Oosterlaan, J., Duivenvoorden, H. J., & Weisglas-Kuperus, N
2009Executive functions in very preterm children at early school age. Journal of Abnormal. Child Psychology 37, 981–999. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Aarnoudse-Moens, C. S., Duivenvoorden, H. J., Weisglas-Kuperus, N., Van Goudoever, J. B., & Oosterlaan, J.
2012The profile of executive function in very preterm children at 4 to 12 years. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology 54(3), 247–253. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Adams, A.-M.
1996Phonological working memory and spoken language development in young children. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A 49, 216–233. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Adams, A.-M., & Gathercole, S. E.
1995Phonological working memory and speech production in young children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 38, 493–414.Google Scholar
1996Phonological working memory and spoken language development in children. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 49A, 216–233. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Anderson, P. J., & Doyle, L. W.
2004Executive functioning in school-aged children who were born very preterm or with extremely low birth weight in the 1990s. Pediatrics 114(1), 50–57. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Aguado, G.
1999Desarrollo de la morfosintaxis en el niño (TSA). Madrid: CEPE.Google Scholar
Aylward, G. P.
2005Neurodevelopmental outcomes of infants born prematurely. Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics 26(6), 427–440. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baron, I. S., Kerns, K. A., Müller, U., Ahronovich, M. D., & Litman, F. R.
2012Executive functions in extremely low birth weight and late-preterm preschoolers: Effects on working memory and response inhibition. Child Neuropsychology 18, 586–599. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Barre, N., Morgan, A., Doyle, L. W., & Anderson, P. J.
2011Language abilities in children who were very preterm and/or very low birth weight: A meta-analysis. Journal of Pediatrics 158(5), 766-U100. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Beauchamp, M. H., Thompson, D. K., Howard, K., Doyle, L. W., Egan, G. F., Inder, T. E., & Anderson, P. J.
2008Preterm infant hippocampal volumes correlate with later working memory deficits. Brain 131, 2986–2994. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bishop, D.
2003Test for reception of grammar (TROG-2). New York, NY: Pearson.Google Scholar
Bohlmann, N. L., Maier, M. F., & Palacios, N.
2015Bidirectionality in self-regulation and expressive vocabulary: Comparisons between monolingual and dual language learners in preschool. Child Development 86, 1094–1111. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Böhm, B., Smedler, A., & Forssberg, H.
2004Impulse control, working memory and other executive functions preterm children when starting school. Acta Paediatrica 93(10), 1363–1371. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Carlson, S. M.
2005Developmentally sensitive measures of executive function in preschool children. Developmental Neuropsychology 28(2), 595–616. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Blencowe, H., Cousens, S., Chou, D., Oestergaard, M., Say, L., Moller, A.-B., . . . Lawn, J.
2013Born too soon: The global epidemiology of 15 million preterm births. Reproductive Health 10(Suppl. 1), S2, 1–16. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brancal, M., Alcantud, F., Ferrer, A., & Quiroga, M.
2005E.D.A.F. Evaluación de la discriminación auditiva y fonológica. Madrid: TEA Ediciones.Google Scholar
Cattani, A., Bonifacio, S., Fertz, M., Iverson, J. M., Zocconi, E., & Caselli, M. C.
2010Communicative and linguistic development in preterm children: A longitudinal study from 12 to 24 months. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders 45(2), 162–173. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Caldwell, B., & Bradley, R.
1984Home observation for measurement of the environment. Administration manual (Rev. ed.). Little Rock, AR: University of Arkansas at Little Rock.Google Scholar
D'Odorico, L., Assanelli, A., Franco, F., & Jacob, V.
2007A follow-up study on Italian late talkers: Development of language, short-term memory, phonological awareness, impulsiveness, and attention. Applied Psycholinguistics 28(1), 157–169. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dunn, L. M., Dunn, L. M., & Arribas, D.
2005PEABODY. Test de vocabulario en imágenes. Madrid: TEA Ediciones.Google Scholar
Duvall, S. W., Erickson, S. J., MacLean, P., & Lowe, J. R.
2015Perinatal medical variables predict executive function within a sample of preschoolers born very low birth weight. Journal of Child Neurology 30, 735–740. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Farrell-Pagulayan, K., Busch, R. M., Medina, K. L., Bartok, J. A., & Krikorian, R.
2006Developmental normative data for the Corsi Block-Tapping Task. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 28(6), 1043–1052. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Foster-Cohen, S., Edgin, J. O., Champion, P. R., & Woodward, L. J.
2007Early delayed language development in very preterm infants: Evidence from the MacArthur-Bates CDI. Journal of Child Language 34(3), 655–675. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Foster-Cohen, S. H., Friesen, M. D., Champion, P. R., & Woodward, L. J.
2010High prevalence/low severity language delay in preschool children born very preterm. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics 31(8), 658–667. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fuhs, M. W., & Day, J. D.
2011Verbal ability and executive functioning development in preschoolers at Head Start. Developmental Psychology 47, 404–416. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S. E.
2007Working memory and language. In M. G. Gaskell (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of psycholinguistics (pp. 757–769). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D.
1990The role of phonological memory in vocabulary acquisition: A study of young children learning new names. British Journal of Psychology 81, 439–454. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., Service, E., Hitch, G. J., Adams, A. M., & Martin, A. J.
1999Phonological short-term memory and vocabulary development: Further evidence on the nature of the relationship. Applied Cognitive Psychology 13, 65–77. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Guarini, A., & Sansavini, A.
2012Language, executive functions, short-term memory and literacy in preterm children: A longitudinal study. Rivista Di Psicolinguistica Applicata 12(3), 101–115.Google Scholar
Hodel, A. S., Senich, K. L., Jokinen, C., Sasson, O., Morris, A. R., & Thomas, K. M.
2017Early executive function differences in infants born moderate-to-late preterm. Early Human Development 113, 23–30. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Howard, K., Roberts, G., Lim, J., Lee, K. J., Barre, N., Treyvaud, K., et al.
2011Biological and environmental factors as predictors of language skills in very preterm children at 5 years of age. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics 32(3), 239–249. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ibbotson, P., & Kearvell-White, J.
2015Inhibitory control predicts grammatical ability. PLoS One 10(12), e0145030. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Im-Bolter, N., Johnson, J., & Pascual-Leone, J.
2006Processing limitations in children with specific language impairment: The role of executive function. Child Development 77(6), 1822–1841. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kern, S., & Gayraud, F.
2007Influence of preterm birth on early lexical and grammatical acquisition. First Language 27(2), 159–173. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kessels, R. P. C., van Zandvoort, M. J. E., Postma, A., Kappelle, L. J., & de Haan, E. H. F.
2000The Corsi Block-Tapping Task: Standardization and normative data. Applied Neuropsychology 7(4), 252–258. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lowe, J., MacLean, P. C., Shaffer, M. L., & Watterberg, K.
2009Early working memory in children born with extremely low birth weight: Assessed by object permanence. Journal of Child Neurology 24, 410–415. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Luciana, M., Lindeke, L., Georgief, M., Mills, M., & Nelson, C. A.
1999Neurobehavioural evidence for working memory deficits in school-aged children with histories of prematurity. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology 41, 521–533. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Luoma, L., Herrgard, E., Martikainen, A., & Ahonen, T.
1998Speech and language development of children born at <= 32 weeks’ gestation: A 5-year prospective follow-up study. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology 40(6), 380–387. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Luu, T. M., Ment, L., Allan, W., Schneider, K., & Vohr, B. R.
2011Executive and memory function in adolescents born very preterm. Pediatrics 127, e639–e646. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Marlow, N., Hennessy, E. M., Bracewell, M. A., Wolke, D., & Grp, E. P. S.
2007Motor and executive function at 6 years of age after extremely preterm birth. Pediatrics 120(4), 793–804. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Marton, K., Kelmenson, L., & Pinkhasova, M.
2007Inhibition control and working memory capacity in children with SLI. Psychologia 50(2), 110–121. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Marton, K., Campanelli, L., Eichorn, N., Scheuer, J., & Yoon, J.
2014Information processing andproactive interference in children with and without specific language impairment. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research 57(1), 106–119. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Menyuk, P., Liebergott, J., Schultz, M., Chesnick, M., & Ferrier, L.
1991Patterns of early lexical and cognitive development in premature and full-term infants. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 34(1), 88–94. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mendoza, E., Carballo, G., Muñoz, J., & Fresneda, D.
2005CEG: Test de Comprensión de Estructuras Gramaticales. Madrid: TEA Ediciones.Google Scholar
Mikkola, K., Ritari, N., Tommiska, V., Salokorpi, T., Lehtonen, L., Tammela, O., et al.
2005Neurodevelopmental outcome at 5 years of age of a national cohort of extremely low birth weight infants who were born in 1996-1997. Pediatrics 116(6), 1391–1400. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., & Howerter, A.
2000The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology 41, 49–100. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Montgomery, J. W.
2003Working memory and comprehension in children with specific language impairment: What we know so far. Journal of Communication Disorders 36, 221–231. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Moreno, C.
1992Escala H.O.M.E. (Home Observation for Measurement of the Enviroment; Caldwell y Bradley, 1984) y C.V.C. Cuestionario de la Vida Cotidiana) . Sevilla: University of Sevilla.Google Scholar
Mulder, H., Pitchford, N. J., & Marlow, N.
2011Processing speed mediates executive function difficulties in very preterm children in middle childhood. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 17, 445–454. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mulder, H., Pitchford, N. J., Hagger, M. S., & Marlow, N.
2009Development of executive function and attention in preterm children: A systematic review. Developmental Neuropsychology 34, 393–421. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Newborg, J., Stock, J. R., & Wnek, L.
1996Batelle, inventario de desarrollo. Madrid: TEA Ediciones.Google Scholar
Ni, T. L., Huang, C. C., & Guo, N. W.
2011Executive function deficit in preschool children born very low birth weight with normal early development. Early Human Development 87(2), 137–141. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nigg, J. T.
2006What causes ADHD? Understanding what goes wrong and why. New York, NY: Guilford.Google Scholar
Noort-van der Spek, I. van Franken, M., & Weisglas-Kuperus, N.
2012Language functions in preterm-born children: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Pediatrics 129(4), 745–754. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pérez-Pereira, M., & Cruz, R.
2018A longitudinal study of vocabulary size and composition in low risk preterm children. First Language, 38(1), 72–94. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Perez-Pereira, M., Fernandez, P., Luisa Gomez-Taibo, M., & Resches, M.
2014Language development of low risk preterm infants up to the age of 30 months. Early Human Development 90(10), 649–656. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pérez Pereira, M., Peralbo, M., & Veleiro, A.
2017 Executive functions and language development in pre-term and full-term children. In A. Auza-Benavides & R. G. Schwartz (Eds.), Language development and disorders in Spanish-speaking children (pp. 91–112). New York, NY: Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Réveillon, M., Borradori Tolsa, C., Monnier, M., Hüppi, P. S., & Barisnikov, K.
2015Response inhibition difficulties in preterm children aged 9–12 years: Relations with emotion and behavior. Child Neuropsychology 22(4), 420–422. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ritter, B. C., Perrig, W., Steinlin, M., & Everts, R.
2014Cognitive and behavioral aspects of executive functions in children born very preterm. Child Neuropsychology 20, 129–144. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Roello, M., Ferretti, M. L., Colonnello, V., & Levi, G.
2015When words lead to solutions: Executive function deficits in preschool children with specific language impairment. Research in Developmental Disabilities 37, 216–222. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rueda, M. R., Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Halparin, J. D., Gruber, D. B., Lercari, L. P., et al.
2004Development of attentional networks in childhood. Neuropsychologia 42(8), 1029–1040. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Saavalainen, P., Luoma, L., Bowler, D., Määttä, S., Kiviniemi, V., Laukkanen, E., et al.
2007Spatial span in very prematurely born adolescents. Developmental Neuropsychology 32, 769–785. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sansavini, A., Guarini, A., Alessandroni, R., Faldella, G., Giovanelli, G., & Salvioli, G.
2006Early relations between lexical and grammatical development in very immature Italian preterms. Journal of Child Language 33(1), 199–216. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2007Are early grammatical and phonological working memory abilities affected by preterm birth? Journal of Communication Disorders 40(3), 239–256. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Scott, M. N., Taylor, H. G., Fristad, M. A., Klein, N., Espy, K. A., Minich, N., & Hack, M.
2012Behavior disorders in extremely preterm/extremely low birth weight children in Kindergarten. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics 33, 202–213. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Taylor, J.
2002Cognitive grammar. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Ullman, M. T.
2001A neurocognitive perspective on language: The declarative/ procedural model. Neuroscience 2, 717–725.Google Scholar
Vissers, C., Koolen, S., Hermans, D., Scheper, A., & Knoors, H.
2015Executive functioning in preschoolers with specific language impairment. Frontiers in Psychology 6. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Weiland, C., Barata, M. C., & Yoshikawa, H.
2014The co-occurring development of executive function skills and receptive vocabulary in children enrolled in an urban preschool program. Infant and Child Development 23, 4–21. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vugs, B., Hendriks, M., Cuperus, J., & Verhoeven, L.
2014Working memory performance and executive function behaviors in young children with SLI. Research in Developmental Disabilities 35(1), 62–74.
https://doi.org/
Google Scholar
White, L. J., Alexander, A., & Greenfield, D. B.
2017The relationship between executive functioning and language: Examining vocabulary, sintax and language learning in preschoolers attending Head Start. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 164, 16–31.
https://doi.org/
. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Woodward, L. J., Edgin, J. O., Thompson, D., & Inder, T. E.
2005Object working memory deficits by early brain injury and development in the preterm infant. Brain 128, 2578–2587. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Woodward, L. J., Clark, C. A. C., Bora, S., & Inder, T. E.
2012Neonatal White Matter abnormalities an important predictor of neurocognitive outcome for very preterm children. PloS One 7(12), e51879. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 3 other publications

Bertin, Tiphanie, Caroline Masson, Marine Le Mené Guigourès & Christine da Silva-Genest
2024. Évaluation des compétences communicatives des enfants nés prématurés avant deux ans : complémentarité des données induites et spontanées. Éla. Études de linguistique appliquée N° 210:2  pp. 161 ff. DOI logo
Pérez-Pereira, Miguel, Zeltia Martínez-López & Lorena Maneiro
2020. Longitudinal Relationships Between Reading Abilities, Phonological Awareness, Language Abilities and Executive Functions: Comparison of Low Risk Preterm and Full-Term Children. Frontiers in Psychology 11 DOI logo
ÖZDEMİR, Salih C., Aslı AKTAN-ERCİYES & Tilbe GÖKSUN
2023. Parental use of causal language for preterm and full-term children: A longitudinal study. Journal of Child Language  pp. 1 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 28 march 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.