Part of
Coherence
T. Givón
[Not in series 230] 2020
► pp. 261286
Abdulaev, Y. G. and M. I. Posner
(1997) “Time-course of activating brain areas in generating verbal associations”, Psychological Science, 8.1. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Andersen, R.
(1979) “Expanding Schumann’s pidginization hypothesis”, Language Learning, 29. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Anderson, A.S.C. Garrod and A. J. Sanford
(1983) “The accessibility of pronominal antecedents as a function of episodic shift in narrative text”, Quarterly J. of Experimental Psychology, 35AGoogle Scholar
Aquinas, St. Thomas Summa Theologica, Benziger Brothers Edition
(1947), tr. by Fathers of the English Dominican Province [public domain, on-line edition]Google Scholar
Aristotle, De Interpretatione
, in J. Barnes (ed. 1984).Google Scholar
Aristotle De Partibus Animalum
, in J. Barnes (ed. 1984).Google Scholar
Aristotle, De Partibus Animalium
, in R. McKeon (ed. 1941)Google Scholar
Aristotle, Posterior Analytic
; in J. Barnes (ed. 1984) DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Aristotle, Prior Analytic
; in J. Barnes (ed. 1984) DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Atkinson, R. C. and R. M. Shiffrin
(1968) “Human memory: A proposed system and its control processes”, in K. W. Spence and T. Spence (eds) The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, vol. 2, NY: Academic Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Augustine, St. The City of God
tr. by H. Bettenson, NY: Penguin Classics (ppbk 1972).Google Scholar
Austin, J.
(1962) “How to do things with words”, Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Austin, P.
(1992) “Case and clauses in Jiwarli, Western Australia”: La Trobe University Working Papers in Linguistics #5, Bundoora, Vic.Google Scholar
Bach, E.
(1965) “Structural linguistics and the philosophy of science”, Diogenes, 51.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. D.
(1986) Working Memory, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Badre, D. and A. D. Wagner
(2007) “The left ventro-lateral prefrontal cortex and the cognitive control of memory”, Neuropsychologia, 45. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bahlmann, J., R. I. Schubotz and A. Friederici
(2008) “Hierarchical artificial grammar processing engages Broca’s area”, NeuroImage 42,2. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baldwin, J. M.
(1896) “A new factor in evolution”, American Naturalist, 30.Google Scholar
Barker, M.
(2003) “The representation of conversation in episodic memory: Information vs. interaction”, TR no. 03-1, Institute of Cognitive and Decision Sciences, University of Oregon, Eugene. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2004) Effects of Divided Attention on Verbal Episodic Memory, PhD dissertation, University of Oregon, Eugene (ms).Google Scholar
Barker, M. and T. Givón
(2002) “On the pre-linguistic origins of language processing rates”, in T. Givón and B. F. Malle (eds 2002) DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2003) “The representation of conversation in episodic memory: Information vs. interaction”, TR no. 03-1, Institute of Cognitive and Decision Sciences, University of Oregon.Google Scholar
Barkow, J. H., L. Cosmites and J. Tooby
(1992) The Adapted Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and the Generation of Culture, Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Barnes, J.
(ed. 1984) The Complete Works of Aristotle, Bolingen Series LXXI, Princeton, N.J.: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S.
(1995) Mindblindness: An essay on Autism and Theory of Mind, Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2000) “The evolution of a theory of mind”, in M. Corballis and S. E. G. Lea (eds) The Descent of Mind, Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bartsch, K. and H. M. Wellman
(1995) Children Talk about the Mind, Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Basso, K. H.
(1972) “To give up words: Silence in Western Apache culture”, in P. P. Giglioni (ed.) Language and Social Contexts: Selected Readings, London: Penguin.Google Scholar
Bentivoglio, P.
(1983) “Topic continuity and discontinuity in discourse: A study in spoken Latin-American Spanish”, in T. Givón (ed. 1983) DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bickerton, D.
(1981) Roots of Language, Ann Arbor, Mich.: Karoma. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1990) Language and Species, Chicago: University of Chicago Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2005) “The origin of language in niche construction”, Honolulu, HI: University of Hawaii at Manoa (ms.). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2008) Bastard Tongues, NY: Hill and Wang.Google Scholar
Bickerton, D. and C. Odo
(1976) Change and Variation in Hawaiian English, vol. I: The Pidgin, NSF Final Report (grant GS-39748), Honolulu: University of Hawaii (ms).Google Scholar
Bickerton, D. and T. Givón
(1976) “Pidginization and syntactic change”, in CLS 12, University of Chicago: Chicago Linguistics Society. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bloom, L.
(1973) One Word at a Time: The Use of Single Word Utterances Before Syntax, The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Bloomfield, L.
(1922) “Review of E. Sapir’s Language ”, The Classical Weekly, 18.Google Scholar
(1933) Language, NY: Holt, Rinehart and Winston. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Boesch, C.
(2002) “Cooperative hunting roles among Tai chimpanzees”, Human Nature, 13. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2005) “Joint cooperative hunting among chimpanzees: Taking natural observations seriously”, Behavior and Brain Science, 28, (comments on Tomasello et al. 2005) DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Boesch, C. and H. Boesch-Achermann
(2000) The Chimpanzees of Tai Forest: Behavioral Ecology and Evolution, Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bonner, J. T.
(1988) The Evolution of Complexity by Means of Natural Selection, Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
(2004) Size Matters, Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Brody, B.
(ed. 1970) Readings in the Philosophy of Science, Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bowerman, M.
(1973) Early Syntactic Development, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bowman, C. R. and D. Zeithamova
(2018) “Abstract memory representation in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex and hippocampus support concept generalization”, J. of Neuroscience, 38.10, (March 2018) DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Boyd, R. and P. R. Richerson
(1985) Culture and the Evolutionary Process, Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Bräuer, J., J. Call and M. Tomasello
Brewer, M. B.
(1981) “Ethnocentrism and its role in interpersonal trust”, in M. B. Brewer and G. E. Collins (eds) Scientific Inquiry and the Social Sciences, San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brown, C.
(1983) “Topic continuity in written English”, in T. Givón (ed. 1983) DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bourke, V. J.
(ed. 1960) The Pocket Aquinas, NY: Washington Square Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bugnyar, T.
(2011) “Knower-guesser differentiation in ravens: Others’ viewpoint matters”, Proc. of the Royal Soc. of London, B. Biological Sci., 273.Google Scholar
(2013) “”Social cognition in ravens”, Compar. Cognition and Behavior Review, 8.Google Scholar
Bugnyar, T. and B. Heinrich
(2005) “Ravens, Corvus corax, differentiate between knowledgeable and ignorant competitors”, Proc. Royal Soc of London, B. Bioilogical Sci., 272.Google Scholar
Bugnyar, T., S. A. Reber and C. Buckner
(2016) “Ravens attribute visual access to unseen competitors”, Natural Communication, 7. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Burt, A. and R. Trivers
(2006) Genes in Conflict, Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bybee, J., W. Pagliuca and R. Perkins
(1994) The Evolution of Grammar: Tense, Aspect and Modality in the Languages of the World, Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Byrne, R. W.
(1998) “Learning by imitation: A hierarchic approach”, Behavior and Brain Sciences, 21:667–721. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Byrne, R. W. and A. Whiten
(eds 1988) Machiavelian Intelligence: Social Expertise and the Evolution of Intellect in Monkeys, Apes and Humans, Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Campbell, D. T.
(1975) “On conflicts between biological and social evolution and between psychology and moral tradition”, The American Psychologist, 30. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Caporeal, L. R., R. M. Dawes, J. M. Orbell and A.J.C. van de Kragt
(1989) “Selfishness examined: Cooperation in the absence of egoistic incentives”, Brain and Behavioral Sciences, 12.4.Google Scholar
Carnap, R.
(1963) The Philosophy of Rudolph Carnap, ed. by P. A. Schilpp, La Sall, IL: Open Court. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chafe, W.
(1994) Discourse, Consciousness and Time: Displacement of Conscious Experience in Speaking and Writing, Chicago: University of Chicago Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1997) “Polyphonic topic development”, in T. Givón (ed. 1997a) DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chase, W. G. and A. Ericsson
(1982) “Skill and working memory”, in G. Bower (ed.) The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 6, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Chase, W. G. and H. Simon
(1973) “Perception in chess”, Cognition, 4.Google Scholar
Cheney, D. L. and R. M. Seyfarth
(1990) How Monkeys See the World, Chicago: University of Chicago Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cheney, D. and R. Seyfarth
(2007) Baboon Metaphysics, Chicago: University of Chicago Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N.
(1961) “On the notion ‘rule of grammar’”, The Structure of Language and its Mathematical Aspects, Providence, RI: American Mathematical Society. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1965) Aspect of the Theory of Syntax, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
(1968) Language and Mind, NY: Harcourt, Brace and World. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1973) For Reasons of State, NY: Pantheon (ppbk).Google Scholar
Clayton, N. S., J. M. Daly and N. J. Emery
(2007) “Social cognition by food-caching corvids: The western scrub-jay as a natural psychologist”, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Soc. of London, B. Biological Sci., 362. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Coates, J.
(1997) “The construction of collaborative floor in women’s friendly talk”, in T. Givón (ed. 1997a) DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cole, P. and J. Morgan
(eds 1975) Speech Acts, Syntax and Semantics, 3, New York: Academic Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Collins, A. M. and E. F. Loftus
(1975) “A spreading activation theory of semantic processing”, Psych. Review, 82. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Collins, A. M. and M. R. Quillian
(1969) “Retrieval time from semantic memory”, J.V.L.V.B., 8.Google Scholar
(1972) “How to make a language user”, in E. Tulvig and W. Donaldson (eds) Organization of Memory, NY: Academic Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Comrie, B. and H. J. Davis
(2015) “Switch-reference in Kobon and Haruai: Areal influences within Highland New Guinea”, Seminario de Complejidad Sintáctica, Hermosillo: Universidad de Sonora, Nov. 2015 (ms.)Google Scholar
Confucius, The Analects
tr. and annotated with an Introduction by A. Waley (1938), NY: Vintage (ppbk 1989).Google Scholar
Crockford, C., R. M. Wittig, R. Mundry and K. Zuberbühler
(2012) “Wild chimpanzees inform ignorant group members of danger”, Current Biology, 22. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Crockford, C., R. M. Wittig and K. Zuberbühler
(2015) “An intentional vocalization draws other’ attention: A playback experiment with wild chimpanzees”, Animal Cognition, 18. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2017) “Vocalizing in chimpanzees is influence by social-cognitive processes”, Science Advances, 3.11 (Nov. 2017) DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Croft, W., K. Denning and S. Kemmer
(eds 1990) Studies in Typology and Diachrony, For Joseph Greenberg, TSL #20, Amsterdam: J. Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Crouch, J. E.
(1978) Functional Human Anatomy, 3rd edition, Philadelphia: Lea and Fabiger.Google Scholar
Daly, J. M., N. J. Emery and N. S. Clayton
(2006) “Food-caching western scrub-jays keep track of who was watching when”, Science, 312.Google Scholar
d’Andrade, E. N.
(1954) Sir Isaac Newton: His life and Work, Garden City, NY: Doubleday.Google Scholar
Darwin, C.
(1859) On the Origin of Species by Means of Natural Selection, or The Preservation of Favoured Races in the Struggle for Life, in E. O. Wilson (ed. 2006) DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1871) The Descent of Man, and Selection in Relation to Sex, in E. O. Wilson (ed. 2006) DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Davis, H. J.
(1987) Kobon Syntax, PhD dissertation, Cambridg University: St. John’s College, (ms.). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dawes, R. M., J. M. Orbell, R. T. Simmons and A.J.C. van de Kragt
(1986) “Organizing groups for collective action”, American Political Science Review, vol…. 1171–1185.. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dawes, R. M., J. McTavish and H. Shaklee
(1977) “Behavior, communication, and assumptions about other people’s behavior in a commons dilemma situation”, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 35. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dawkins, R.
(1976) The Selfish Gene, NY: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Derbyshire, D. C. and J. Pullum
(eds 1998) Handbook of Amazonian Languages, vol. 4. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
de Waal, F.
(1982) Chimpanzee Politics: Power and Sex among the Apes, London: Unwin Paperbacks/Counterpoint. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1989) Peace Making Among Primates, Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2013) The Bonobo and the Atheist, NY: Norton.Google Scholar
de Waal, F. and F. Lanting
(1997) Bonobo: The Forgotten Ape, Berkeley: University of California Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Diamond, J.
(2005) Collapse, NY: Viking.Google Scholar
Dickenson, C. and T. Givón
(1997) “Memory and conversation”, in T. Givón (ed. 1997a) DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Diogenes Laertius Lives of the Eminent Philosophers
Tr. by R. D. Hicks (1925), 2 vols; The Loeb Classical Library, Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
DuBois, J.
(1987) “The discourse basis of ergativity”, Language, 63.4. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dunbar, R. I. M.
(1992) “Co-evolution of neo-cortex size, group size and language in humans”, Brain and Behavior Sciences, 16.4. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dunbar, R. M.
(1998) “The social brain hypothesis”, Evolutionary Anthropology, 6.5. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Eckert, R. & D. Randall
(1978) Animal Physiology, 2nd edition, NY/San Francisco: W.H. Freeman.Google Scholar
Elster, J.
(ed. 1985) The Divided Self, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ericsson, A. and W. Kintsch
(1997) “Long term working memory”, Psychological Review, 102.2. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ervin-Tripp, F. and A. Kuntay
(1997) “The occasioning and structure of conversational stories”, in T. Givón (ed. 1997a) DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fan, J.
(2014) “An information theory account of cognitive control”, Human Neuroscience 8, September. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fedden, S.
(2011) A Grammar of Mian, Berlin: De Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fernald, R. D. and S. A. White
(2000) “Social control of brain: From behavior to genes”, in M. Gazzaniga (ed.) The New Cognitive Neuroscience, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Feyerabend, P.
(1975) Against Method: An Outline of an Anarchic Theory of Knowledge, NY: The Humanities Press.Google Scholar
Flombaum, J. I. and L. R. Santos
(2005) “Rhesus monkeys attribute perception to others”, Current Biology, 15. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fouts, R. S.
(1973) “Acquisition and testing of gestural signs in four young chimpanzees, Science, 180. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fox, A.
(1983) “Topic continuity in Biblical Hebrew”, in T. Givón (ed. 1983) DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Frank, P.
(1985) A Grammar of Ika, Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania, PhD dissertation (ms.).. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Friederici, A.
(2008) “Brain circuits of syntax: Fron neuro-theoretical considerations to empirical tests”, Ernst Stüngmann Forum on the Biological Foundations and Origin of Syntax, Frankfurt, July 2008 (ms.).Google Scholar
Friederici, A. and S. Frisch
(2000) “Verb-argument structure processing: The role of verb-specific and argument-specific information”, J. of Memory and Language, 43. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fussell, S. R. and R. J. Kreuz
(eds 1998) Social and Cognitive Approaches to Interpersonal Communication, Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Futuyma, D.
(1986) Evolutionary Biology, 2nd edition, Sunderland, MA: Sinauer & Assoc. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gardner, B. T. and R. A. Gardner
(1971) “Two-way communication with an infant chimpanzee”, in A. Schrier and F. Stollnitz (eds) Behavior of Non-human Primates, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Gat, A.
(1999) “The pattern of fighting in simple, small-scale, pre-state societies”, J. of Anthropological Research, 55. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2006) War in Human Civilization, Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gathercole, S. E. and A. D. Baddeley
(1993) Working Memory and Language, Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Gazzaniga, M. S.
(2005) The Ethical Brain, NY & Washington, DC: Dana Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gernsbacher, M. A.
(1985) “Surface information loss in comprehension”, Cognitive Psychology, 17. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1990) Language Comprehension as Structure Building, Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(ed. 1994) Handbook of Psycholinguistics, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Gernsbacher, M. A. and T. Givón
(eds 1994) Coherence in Spontaneous Discourse, TSL #31, Amsterdam: J. Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Givón, T.
(1971) “Historical syntax and synchronic morphology: An archaeologist’s field trip”, CLS #6, Chicago: University of Chicago, Chicago Linguistics Society. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1973a) “Opacity and reference in language: An inquiry into the role of modalities”, in J. Kimball (ed.) Syntax and Semantics, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
(1973b) Studies in Chi-Bemba and Bantu Grammar, Studies in African Linguistics, Supplement #3.Google Scholar
(1976) “Topic, pronoun and grammatical agreement”, in C. Li (ed. 1976).Google Scholar
(1977) “The drift from VSO to SVO in Biblical Hebrew: The pragmatics of Tense-Aspect”, in C. Li (ed. 1977).Google Scholar
(1979/2019) On Understanding Grammar, revised edition; Amsterdam: J. Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1983a) “Topic continuity in discourse: Introduction”, in T. Givón (ed. 1983) DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1983b) “Topic continuity is spoken English” in T. Givón (ed. 1983) DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1983c) “Topic continuity in Ute”, in T. Givón (ed. 1983).Google Scholar
(ed. 1983) Topic Continuity in Discourse: Quantified Cross-Language Studies, TSL #3, Amsterdam: J. Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1984b) “Universals of discourse structure and second language acquisition”, in W. Rutherford (ed.) Language Universals and Second Language Acquisition, TSL #5, Amsterdam: J. Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1988) “The pragmatics of word-order: Predictability, importance and attention”, in E. Moranvcsik et al. (eds 1988) DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1989) Mind, Code and Context: Essays in Pragmatics, Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
(1990) “Natural language learning and organized language teaching”, in H. Burmeister and P. Rounds (eds) Variability in Second Language Acquisition: Proceedings of the 10th Second Language Research Forum (SLRF), Eugene: University of Oregon.Google Scholar
(1991a) “Serial verbs and the mental reality of ‘even’”, in E. Traugott and B. Heine (eds 1991, vol. 1) DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1991b) “Isomorphism in the grammatical code: Cognitive and biological considerations”, Studies in Language, 15.1. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1992) “The grammar of referential coherence as mental processing instructions”, Linguistics, 30.1. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1995) Functionalism and Grammar, Amsterdam: J. Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(ed. 1997a) Conversation: Cultural, Cognitive, and Communicative Perspectives, TSL #34, Amsterdam: J. Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(ed. 1997b) Grammatical relations: A Functionalist perspective, TSL #35, Amsterdam: J. Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2001) Syntax: An Introduction, Amsterdam: J. Benjamins.Google Scholar
(2002) Bio-Linguistics: The Santa Barbara Lectures, Amsterdam: J. Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2009) The Genesis of Syntactic Complexity, Amsterdam: J. Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2013) Ute Texts, Amsterdam: J. Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2015a) The Diachrony of Grammar, Amsterdam: J. Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2015b) “Toward a diachrony of adverbial clauses”, Seminario de Complejidad Sintáctica, Universidad de Sonora, Hermosillo (ms.). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2017a) The Story of Zero, Amsterdam: J. Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2017b) “Tale of two cultures”, Durango Herald, OpEd Section, Sept. 9/10 2017 DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2017c) “Are we a warlike species?” talk to the Life Long Learning forum, Ft. Lewis College, Durango, CO (March 2017).Google Scholar
Givón, T., P. Casías, V. Peabody and M. I. Cloud
(2015) Ute Dictionary, Amsterdam: J. Benjamins.Google Scholar
Givón, T., W. Kellog, P. Yee and M. I. Posner
(1985) “The identification of referents in connected discourse: Automatic vs. attended processing”, Cognitive Science Working Papers, Inst. for Cognitive and Decision Sciences, University of Oregon.Google Scholar
Givón, T. and B. F. Malle
(eds 2002) The Evolution of Language out of Pre-Language, TSL #53, Amsterdam: J. Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Givón, T. and M. Shibatani
(eds 2009) Syntactic Complexity, TSL #85, Amsterdam: J. Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goffman, E.
(1974) Frame Analysis, Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Goodale, M. A.
(2000) “Perception and action in the human visual system”, in M. S. Gazzaniga (ed.) The New Cognitive Neuroscience, 2nd edition, Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goodall, J.
(1965) “Chimpanzees of the Gombe Stream reserve”, in E. deVore (ed) Primate Behavior: Field Studies of Monkeys and Apes, NY: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
Gopnik, A. and H. M. Wellman
(1992) “Why the child’s theory of mind is really a theory”, Mind and Language, 7. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1994) “The theory theory”, in L. A. Hirschfeld and S. A. Gelman (eds) Mapping the Mind: Domain Specificity in Cognition and Culture, NY: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gordon, D. and G. Lakoff
(1975) “Conversational postulates”, in P. Cole and J. Morgan (eds 1975) DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gould, S. J.
(1977) Ontogeny and Phylogeny, Cambridge, Mass: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
(1980) The Panda’s Thumb, London: Pelican. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gray, R. D., M. Heaney and S. Fairhall
(2004) “Evolutionary Psychology and the challenge of adaptive explanation”, in K. Sterelny and J. Fitness (eds) From Mating to Mentality: Evaluating Evolutionary Psychology, NY: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Greenblatt, S.
(2011) The Swerve, NY/London: Norton.Google Scholar
Grice, H. P.
(1968/1975) “Logic and conversation,” in P. Cole and J. Morgan (eds 1975) DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grimes, J.
(1975) The Thread of Discourse, The Hague: Mouton. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Guillhaum, A.
(2000) “Participials, case agreement and switch-reference in Cavineña”, University of Oregon, Eugene (ms.)Google Scholar
Haeckel, E.
(1866) Generelle Morphologie der Organismen, Berlin: Georg Reiner. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Haeckel, F.
(1874) “Die Gastrae Theorie die Phylogenetische Klassifikazion der Tierreischen und der Homologie der Keimlaetter:, Jaenische Zetzshcrift für Naturwissenschaft, 9. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Haiman, J.
(1983) “On some origins of switch-reference marking”, in J. Haiman and P. Munro, (eds 1983) DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1985) Natural Syntax, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(ed. 1985) Iconicity in Syntax, TSL #6, Amsterdam: J. Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Haiman, J. and P. Munro
(eds 1983) Switch Reference and Universal Grammar, TSL #2, Amsterdam: J. Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hale, K.
(1988) “Misumalpan verb sequencing constructions”, Annual Meeting of the Amrican Anthropological Association, Phoenix, AZ: November (ms.). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1991) “Misumalpan verb-sequencing constructions”, in C. Lefebvre (ed. 1991) DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Halliday, M. A. K. and R. Hassan
(1976) Cohesion in English, English Language Series, 9, London: Longman’s.Google Scholar
Halperin, J. M. and K. P. Schulz
(2006) “Revisiting the role of the prefrontal cortex in the pathophysiology of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder”. Psychol. Bull.: 132:560–81. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hamilton, W. D.
(1964) “The genetic evolution of social behavior”, J. of Theoretical Biology, 7.1. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hanson, R. N.
(1958) Patterns of Discovery, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hare, B., J. Call and M. Tomasello
(2000) “Chimpanzees know what conspecifics do and do not see”, Animal Behavior, 59. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Healey, P.
(1966) Levels and Chaining in Sentences, Linguistics Circle of Canberra Publications, Series B, Monograph #5, Canberra: Australian National University.Google Scholar
Heine, B., U. Claudi and F. Hünnemeyer
(1991) Grammaticalization: A Conceptual Framework, Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Heine, B. and T. Kuteva
(2007) The Genesis of Grammar, Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Heller, N.
(2017) “The digital republic”, The New Yorker, 12/18-25/17.Google Scholar
Hempel, C.
(1959) “The logic of functional analysis”, in L. Gross (ed.) Symposium on Sociological Theory, NY: Harper & Row (in B. Brody ed. 1970) DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hempel, C. and P. Oppenheim
(1948) “Studies in the logic of explanation”, Philosophy of Science, XV (in B. Brody ed. 1970) DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Heyes, C. M.
(1998) “Theory of mind in non-human primates”, Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2014) “Animal mind-reading: What’s the problem?”, Psychology Bulletin Review, 22. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hinds, J.
(1983) “Topic Continuity in Japanese”, in T. Givón (ed. 1983) DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hobbes, T.
(1651) Leviathan, Sweden: Wisehouse Classics [2016] DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hopper, P. and E. Traugott
(1993) Grammaticalization, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hrdy, S. B.
(1999) Mother Nature, NY: Pantheon. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2009) Mothers and Others, Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Humphreys, G. W. and J. M. Riddoch
(1987) “Introduction: Cognitive psychology and visual object processing”, in G. W. Humphreys and J. M. Riddoch (eds 1987) DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(eds 1987) Visual Object Processing: A Cognitive Neuropsychological Approach, London: Erlbaum. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Humphreys, G. W. and M. J. Riddoch
(1988) “On the case of multiple semantic systems: A reply to Shallice”, Cognitive Neuropsychology, 5.1. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Huxley, A.
(1932) Brave New World, NY: Harper. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Itkonen, E.
(1983) Causality in Linguistic Theory, London: Croom-Helm.Google Scholar
(2010) “Philosophy of Linguistics”, Oxford Encyclopedia of Philosophy of Science, Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Johnson, N. S. and J. M. Mandler
(1977) “Remembrance of things parsed: Story structure and recall”, Cognitive Psychology, 9. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Johnson, K. A., I. H. Robertson, E. Barry
, et al. (2008) “Impaired conflict resolution and alerting in children with ADHD: evidence from the ANT”. J. Child Psychol Psychiatry: 49:1339–47. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kaas, J. H.
(1989) “Why does the brain have so many visual areas?”, J. of Cognitive Neuroscience, 1.2:121–135. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kahnemann, D.
(1973) Attention and Effort, Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Kahneman, D. and A. M. Treisman
(1984) “Changing views of attention and automaticity”, in D. Parasuraman (ed.) Varieties of Attention, London: Academic Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kant, I.
(1781) Critique of Pure Reason.Google Scholar
Kelly, R. L.
(2007) The Foraging Spectrum: Diversity in Hunter-Gatherer Lifeways, NY: Percheron Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kemp, J.
(1968) The Philosophy of Kant, Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kern, J. M. and A. N. Radford
(2017) “Reduced social-information provision by immigrants and use by residents following dispersal”, Current Biology, 27.13 (Dec. 4 2017).Google Scholar
Kintsch, W.
(1982) “Memory for text”, in A. Flammer and W. Kintsch (eds) Text Processing, Amsterdam: North Holland.Google Scholar
(1992) “How readers construct situation models for stories: The role of syntactic cues and causal inference”, in A. F. Healy, S. Kosslyn and R. M. Shiffrin (eds) Essays in Honor of William K. Estes, Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1994) “The psychology of discourse processing”, in M. A. Gernsbacher (ed. 1994) DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kintsch, W. and T. van Dijk
(1978) “Toward a model of text comprehension and production”, Psychological Review, 85. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Klein, R. G.
(1989) The Human Career: Human Biological and Cultural Origins, Chicago: University of Chicago Press [2nd edition 1999] DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Koestler, A.
(1969) The Ghost in the Machine, rep. 1990; London/NY: Penguin. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kuhn, T.
(1962) The Structure of Scientific Revolutions, Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Lakatos, I.
(1978) The Methodology of Scientific Research Programmes, Philosophical Papers, vol. I, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lamarck, J.-B.
(1809) Philosophie Zoologique, ou Exposition des Considerations Relative a l’Histoire Naturelle des Animaux, Paris; English translation by H. Elliot, The Zoological Philosophy, London: Macmillan [1914]Google Scholar
Lamendella, J.
(1976) “Relations between the ontogeny and phylogeny of language: A neo-recapitulationist view”, in S. R. Harnad, S. D. Stelkis and J. Lancaster (eds), The Origins and Evolution of Language and Speech, NY: New York Academy of Science. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
L’Amour, L.
(1962) Shalako, NY: Bantam (ppbk). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Landry, R. and S. E. Bryson
(2004) “Impaired disengagement of attention in young children with autism”. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2004;45:1115–22. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lao Tse Tao Teh Ching (Book of the Tao)
tr. with a Retroduction by T. Givón, Ignacio, CO: White Cloud Publishing (2012) DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Leach, E. R.
(1958) “Concerning Trobriand clans and the kinship category Tabu ”, in J. Goody (ed.) The Developmental Cycle in Domestic Groups, Cambridge Papers in Social Anthropology, 1, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lees, R. B.
(1957) “Review of Chomsky’s Syntactic Structures ”, Language, 33. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Leonard, R.
(2017) “Why rural America voted for Trump”, NY Times, Op-Ed section (online), 1-5-17.Google Scholar
Leslie, A. M. and U. Frith
(1988) “Autistic children’s understanding of seeing, knowing and believing”, British J. of Developmental Psychology, 6. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lefebvre, C.
(ed. 1991) Serial Verbs: Grammatical, Comparative and Cognitive Approaches, SSLS #8, Amsterdam: J. Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Levinson, S.
(2000) Presumptive Meaning, Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Li, C. N.
(ed 1976) Subject and Topic, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
(ed 1977) Mechanisms of Syntactic Change, Austin: University of Texas Press.Google Scholar
(2002) “Missing links, issues and hypotheses in the evolutionary origin of language”, in T. Givón and B. F. Malle (eds 2002) DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Libet, B.
(1991) “Conscious vs. neural time”, Nature, 352 (6330): 27–28. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1999) “Do we have free will?”, J. of Consciousness Studies, 6. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lieberman, P.
(1984) The Biology and Evolution of Language, Cambridge, Mass: Harvard University Press.. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Linell, P. and N. Korolija
(1997) “Coherence in multi-party conversation: Episodes and contexts in interaction”, in T. Givón (ed. 1997a) DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Loftus, E.
(1980) Eyewitness Testimony, Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Longacre, R. E.
(1972a) Hierarchy and Universality of Discourse Constituents in New Guinea Languages; vol. 1 Discussion; Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1972b) Hierarchy and Universality of Discourse Constituents in New Guinea Languages; vol. 2, Texts; Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Longacre, R.
(1976) Anatomy of Speech Notions, Lisse: Peter de Ridder Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lounsbury, F. G.
(1965) “Another view of the Trobriand kinship categories”, American Anthropologist, 67.5:2. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lucretius, Titus
, De Rerum Naturae (On the Nature of Things) Tr. W. H. D. Rouse/M. F. Smith, Loeb Classical Library, Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press (1924/1975).Google Scholar
Malinowski, B.
(1932) The Sexual Life of Savages in Northwestern Melanesia, 3rd edition, London: Routledge. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1935) Coral Gardens and Their Magic, London: Allen & Unwin.Google Scholar
Malle, B., L. J. Moses and D. A. Baldwin
(eds 2000) Intentionality: A Key to Human Understanding, Cambridge: MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Malthus, T. R.
(1798) An Essay on the Principle of Population.Google Scholar
Mandler, J. M.
(1978) “A code in the node: The use of story schemata in retrieval”, Discourse Processes, 1. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Margulis, L.
(1981) Symbiosis in Cell Evolution, San Francisco: W.H. Freeman & Co.Google Scholar
Marlowe, F. W.
(2005) “Hunter-gatherers and human evolution”, Evolutionary Anthropology, 14. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2010) The Hadza Hunter-Gatherers of Tanzania, Berkeley: University of California Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mayr, E.
(1963) Animal Species and Evolution, Cambridge., MA: Harvard University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1976) Evolution and the Diversity of Life, Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1982) The Growth of Biological Thought, Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
McCandliss, B., M. I. Posner and T. Givón
(1999) “Brain plasticity in learning visual words”, Cognitive Psychology.Google Scholar
McKaughan, N.
(ed 1973) The Languages of the Easten Family of the East New Guinea Highlands Stock, vol. 1, Seattle: University of Washington Press.Google Scholar
McKeon, R.
(ed. 1941) The Basic Works of Aristotle, NY: Random House [22nd edition] DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N.
(1999) “Origins of theory of mind, cognition and communication”, J. of Communication Disorders, 32:251–269. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2002a) “Imitation as a mechanism of social cognition: Origins of empathy, theory of mind and the representation of action”, in U. Goswami (ed.) Blackwell Handbook of Child Cognitive Development, Oxford: Blackwell Publishers. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2002b) “Elements of a developmental theory of imitation”, in A. N. Meltzoff and W. Prinz (eds 2002) DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N. and W. Prinz
(eds 2002) The Imitative Mind: Development, Evoluition and Brain Bases, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Menn, L.
(1990) “Agrammatical English”, in L. Menn and L. Oblers (eds. 1990) Agrammatic Aphasia, (3 vols), Amsterdam: J. Benjamins.Google Scholar
Mesulam, M.-M.
(2000) Principles of Behavioral and Cognitive Neurology, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Mesulam, M.
(2012) “The evolving landscape of human cortical connectivity: Facts and inferences”, Neuroimage, 62: 2182–2189. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mishkin, M.
(1978) “Memory in monkeys severely impaired by combined but not by separate removal of amygdala and hippocampus”, Nature, 273. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1982) “A memory system in the monkey”, Philosophical Soc. of London [Biol.], 298.Google Scholar
Mishkin, M., B. Malamut and J. Bachevalier
(1984) “Memories and habits: Two neural systems”, in G. Lynch and J. L. McGaugh (eds), Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Mithen, S.
(1996) The Prehistory of Mind, London: Penguin. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Moravcsik, E., J. Wirth and M. Hammond
(eds 1988) Typology and Language Universals, TSL #16, Amsterdam: J. Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Morton, J., U. Frith and A. M. Leslie
(1991) “The cognitive basis of a biological disorder: Autism”, Trends in Neuroscience, 4. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Munro, P.
(1976) Mojave Syntax, NY/London: Garland Publ.Google Scholar
(1983) Sketch of Pima Grammar, Los Angeles: UCLA (ms.). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Murray, C.
(2012) Coming Apart, NY: Crown Publishers. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1990) “Semantic priming effect in visual word recognition: A selective review of current findings and theories”, in D. Besner and G. Humphreys (eds) Basic Processes in Reading: Visual Word Recognition, Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Ochs, E. and B. Schieffelin
(eds 1979) Developmental Pragmatics, NY: Academic Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ochs, E., B. Schieffelin and M. Platt
(1979) “Propositions across utterances and speakers”, in E. Ochs and B. Scieffelin (eds 1979).Google Scholar
Okasha, S.
(2006) Evolution and the Level of Selection, Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Orbell, J. M. and R. M. Dawes
(1991) “A ‘cognitive miser’ theory of cooperator’s advantage”, The American Political Science Review, 85.2. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Orbell, J. M., A.J.C. van de Kragt and R. M. Dawes
(1988) “Explaining discussion-induced cooperation”, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Orbell, J. M., L. Zheng and M. Mulford
(1994) “Individual experience and the fragmentation of societies”, University of Oregon, Eugene (ms).Google Scholar
Otterbein, K. F.
(2004) How War Began, College Station, TX: Texas A&M Press.Google Scholar
Pawley, A.
(1966) The Structure of Kalam: A Grammar of a New Guinea Highland Language, Aukland, NZ: Aukland University, PhD dissertation (ms.).Google Scholar
(ed. 1997) Papers in Papuan Linguistics, #3, Pacific Linguistics Series, A-87, Canberra: Research School of Pacific and Asian Studies, Australian National University.Google Scholar
Pawley, A. and F. H. Syder
(1975/2000) “The one clause at a time hypothesis”, in H. Riggenbach (ed.) Perspectives on Fluency, Ann Arbor, Mich.: University of Michigan Press.Google Scholar
Peirce, C. S.
(1934) Collected Writings, vol. V, Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
(1940) The Philosophy of Peirce, ed. by J. Buchler, NY: Harcourt, Brace.Google Scholar
Penner, J., U. Frith, A. M. Leslie and S. Leekam
Pepperberg, I. M.
(1999) The Alex Studies: Cognitive and Communicative Abilities of Grey Parrots, Cambridge, Mass: Harvard University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Perret, D. I., H. Harries, P. J. Benson, A. J. Mistlin, A. J. Citty, J. K. Hietanen and J. E. Ortega
Petersen, S. E., P. T. Fox, M. I. Posner, M. A. Mintun and M. E. Raichle
(1988) “Positron emission tomographic studies of the cortical anatomy of single word processing”, Nature, 331. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Petri, H. L. and M. Mishkin
(1994) “Behaviorism, cognitivism and the new psychology of memory”, American Scientist, 82. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Philips, S.
(1974) “Some sources of cultural variablity in the regulation of talk”, Language in Society, 5. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pinker, S.
(1995) The Language Instinct, NY: Harper Perennial.Google Scholar
Plato The Collected Dialogues of Plato, E. Hamilton & H. Cairns
(eds 1961) Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Potts, M. and T. Hayden
(2008) Sex and War, Dallas, TX: BenBella Books. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Popper, K.
(1934/1959) The Logic of Scientific Discovery, revised edition (1968), NY: Harper and Row. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Povinelli, D. J. and S. deBlois
(1992) “Young children’s understanding of knowledge formation in themselves and others”, J. of Comparative Psychology, 106. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Povinelli, D. J. and T. M. Preuss
(1995) “Theory of mind: Evolutionary history of a cognitive specialization”, Trends in Neuroscience, 18.9. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Povinelli, D. J. and T. Eddy
(1996a) “What young chimpanzees know about seeing”, Monographs of the Society of Research on Child Development, 61.247. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1996b) “Factors affecting young chimpanzees’ recognition of attention”, J. of Comparative Psychology, 110. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1996c) “Chimpanzees: Joint visual attention”, Psychological Science, 7. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Povinelli, D. J., K. E. Nelson and S. T. Boysen
(1990) “Inference about guessing and knowing by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)”, J. of Comparative Psychology, 104. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1992) “Comprehension of role reversal in chimpanzees: Evidence of empathy?”, Animal Behavior, 43. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Premack, D.
(1971) “Language in chimpanzee”, Science, 172. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Premack, D. and G. Woodruff
(1978) “Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind?”, Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1.4. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Posner, M. I.
(1978) Chronometric Explorations of Mind, Oxford: Oxfor University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(ed. 2017) The Psychology of Attention, London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Posner, M. I. and R. Klein
(1971) “On the function of consciousness”, Fourth Conference on Attention and Performance, University of Colorado, Boulder (ms).Google Scholar
Posner, M. I. and J. Fan
(2004) “Attention as an organ system”, Dept. of Psychology, University of Oregon (ms.). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Posner, M. I. and S. W. Boies
(1971) “Components of attention”, Psychological Review, 78. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Posner, M. I. and A. Pavese
(1997) “Anatomy of word and sentence meaning”, Colloquium on Neuroimaging of Human Brain Function, M. Posner and M. Raichle, orgs., Nat. Acad. of Sci. USA.Google Scholar
Posner, M. I. and O. Marín
(eds 1985) Attention and Performance, XI, Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Posner, M. I., S. E. Petersen, P. T. Fox and M. E. Raichle
(1988) “Localization of cognitive operations in the human brain”, Science, 240. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Posner, M. I. and M. Rothbart
in press). “Exploring common mechanisms of brain development and adult plasticity in humans and rodents: An essay in honor of Helen Neville” (ms.).
Posner, M. I. and C. R. R. Snyder
(1974) “Attention and cognitive control”, in R. L. Solso (ed.) Information Processing and Cognition: The Loyola Symposium, Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Power, M.
(1991) The Egalitarians: Human and Chimpanzee, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pu, M.-M.
(1997) “Zero anaphora and grammatical relations in Mandarin”, in T. Givón (ed. 1997b) DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pulvermüller, F.
(1999) “Words in the brain’s language”, Behavioral and Breain Sciences, 22. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2003) The Neuroscience of Language, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pulvermüller, F. and O. Hauk
(2006) “Category specific processing of color and form words in the left fronto-temporal cortex”, Cerebral Cortex. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pulvermüller, F. and Y. Shtyrov
(2003) “Automatic processing of grammar in the human brain as revealed in mismatched negativity”, NeuroImage, 20. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pulvermüller, F., Y. Shtyrov and R. P. Carlyon
Quine, W. van O.
(1951) “Two dogmas of Empiricism”, Philosophical Review, 60. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1953) “Reference and Modality”, ch. 8 of From a Logical Point of View, Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Reesink, G.
(2014) “Topic management and clause combination in the Papuan language Usan”, in R. van Gijn, D. Matiás, J. Hammond, S. van Putten and A. Vilacy Galucio (eds.) Information Structure and Reference Tracking in Complex Sentences, Amsterdam: J. Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Richerson, P. J. and R. Boyd
(1989) “A Darwinian theory for the evolution of symbolic cultural traits”, in M. Freilich (ed.) The Relevance of Culture, pp. 120–142, New York: Bergin & Garvey. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2005) Not by Genes Alone: How Culture Transformed Human Evolution, Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Riddoch, M. J. and G.W. and Humphreys
(1987a) “Visual optic processing in a case of optic aphasia”, Cognitive Neuropsychology, 4. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Riddoch, M. J. and G. W. Humphreys
(1987b) “Picture naming”, in G. W. Humphreys and J. M. Riddoch (eds 1987).Google Scholar
Riddoch, M. J., G. W. Humphreys, M. Coltheart and E. Funnell
(1988) “Semantic systems or system? Neuropsychological evidence reexamined”, Cognitive Neuropsychology, 5.1. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ristau, C. A.
(ed. 1991) Cognitive Ethology: The Minds of Other Animals, Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Roberts, J.
(1997) “Switch-reference in Papua New Guinea: A preliminary survey”, in A. Pawley (ed. 1997).Google Scholar
Ross, J. R.
(1967) Constraints on Variables in Syntax, PhD dissertation, MIT (ms.). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rothbart, M. and M. Taylor
(1992) “Category labels and social reality: Do we view social categories as natural kinds?”, in R. Semin and K. Fiedler (eds) Language, Interaction and Social Cognition, London: Sage. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rousseau, J.-J.
(1754) Discourse: What is the Origin of Inequality Among Men, And is it Authorized by Natural Law, Tr. by G. D. H. Cole, Liberty Library [on-line 1998] DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1762) The Social Contract and Other Later Political Writings, V. Gourevitch (ed. 1997), Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Russell, B.
(1908) “Mathematical logic based on a theory of types”, in Russell (1956) DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1911) “The relations between universals and particulars”, in Russell (1956).Google Scholar
(1918) “The philosophy of logical atomism”, in Russell (1956) DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1948) Human Knowledge, London: Routledge.Google Scholar
(1956) Logic and Knowledge, London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Samuelson, R.
(2017) “True verdict on Obama Administration Years away”, The Washington Post, OpEd section, 1-15-17 [online edition].Google Scholar
Sapolsky, R. M.
Savage-Rumbaugh, S. and R. Lewin
(1994) Kanzi: The Ape at the Brink of the Human Mind, NY: Wiley and Sons. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Savage-Rumbaugh, S., J. Murphy, R. A. Sevcik, K. E. Brakke, S. L. Wiliams and D. M. Rumbaugh
Savage-Rumbaugh, S., S. G. Shanker and T. J. Taylor
(1998) Apes, Language and the Human Mind, Oxford and NY: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Scancarelli, J.
(1989) “Marking continuity in Pima and O’odam (Papago)”, U. of Kentucky (ms.). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schieffelin, E.
(1976) The Sorrow of the Lonely and the Burning of the Dancer, New York: St. Martin’s Press.. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schmahmann, J. D., D. N. Pandya, R. Wang, G. Dai, H. E. D’Arceuil, A.J. de Crespigny and V. J. Wideen
Schmelz, M., J. Call and M. Tomasello
(2011) “Chimpanzees know that others make inferences”, Proc. Nat. Acad. of Sci., USA, 108. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schmidt, M. F. H. and M. Tomasello
(2112) “Young children enforce social norms”, Psychological Science, 21.4. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Seyfarth, R. M. and D. L. Cheney
(2012) “Animal cognition: Chimpanzee alarm calls depend on what others know”, Current Biology, 22. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schneider, W.
(1985) “Toward a model of attention and the development of automatic processing”, in M. I. Posner and O. Marin (eds 1985) DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schneider, W. and J. M. Chein
Schneider, W. and R. M. Shiffrin
(1977) “Controlled and automated human information processing, I: Detection, search and attention”, Psychological Review, 84. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schumann, J.
(1976) “Second language acquisition: The pidginization hypothesis”, Language Learning, 26. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1978) The Pidginization Process: A Model for Second Language Acquisition, Rowley, Mass.: Newbury House.Google Scholar
(1985) “Non-syntactic speech in Spanish-English basilang”, in R. Andersen (ed.) Second Language Acquisition: A Cross-Linguistic Perspective, Rowley, Mass.: Newbury House.Google Scholar
Scollon, R.
(1976) Conversations with a One-Year Old Child, Honolulu: University of Hawaii Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Scriven, M.
(1962) “Explanations, predictions and laws”, in H. Feigl and G. Maxwell (eds) Minnesota Studies in the Philosophy of Science, III; reprinted in B. Brody (ed. 1970).Google Scholar
Searle, J.
(1970) Speech Acts, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Selinker, L.
(1972) “Interlanguage”, International Review of Applied Linguistics, 10. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shallice, T.
(1988) From Neuropsychology to Mental Structure, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Silberbauer, G.
(1981) “Hunters/gatherers of the central Kalahari”, in R. Harding and G. Teleki (eds) Omnivorous Primates: Gathering and Hunting in Human Evolution, New York: Columbia University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Simon, H.
(1961) “The architecture of complexity”, Proc. Amer. Philos. Soc., 106.6 (December).Google Scholar
Sleigh, M.
(1989) Protozoa and Other Protists, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Smirnov, O., H. Arrow, D. Kennett and J. Orbell
(2007) “Ancestral war and the evolutionary origins of ‘heroism’”, J. of Politics, 69.4. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Smith, A.
(1759) The Theory of Moral Sentiment, Lexington, KY (2012) DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1776) An Inquiry into the Nature and Causes of the Wealth of Nations, R. H. Campbell and A. S. Skinner, eds, Edinburgh (1978).. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Snyder, A. Z., Y. G. Abdulaev, M. I. Posner and M. E. Raichle
(1995) “Scalp electrical potentials reflect cerebral blood flow responses during processing of written words”, Proc. Nat. Acad of Sci. USA, 92: 1689–1693) DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sparing-Chávez, M.
(1998) “Interclausel reference in Amahuaca”, in D. C. Derbyshire and J. Pullum (eds 1998).Google Scholar
Squire, L. R.
(1987) Memory and Brain, Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
(1992) “Memory and the hippocampus: A synthesis from findings with rats, monkeys and humans”, Psych. Review, 99:195–231. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Squire, L. R. and S. Zola-Morgan
(1991) “The medial temporal lobe memory system”, Science, 253. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Spitzer, M.
(1999) The Mind Within the Net: Models of Learning, Thinking and Acting, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stiglitz, J.
(2015) The Great Divide, NY: Norton. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stiles, D.
(1994) “On evolutionary ecology and cultural realities”, Current Anthropology, 35.4. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Swinney, D. A.
(1979) “Lexical access during sentence comprehension: (Re) consideration of cotext effects”, J.V.L.V.B., 18.Google Scholar
Terrace, H. S.
(1979) Nim, NY: A.A. Knopf.Google Scholar
(1985) “In the beginning there was the “name””, American Psychologist, 40. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Thomas, D. J.
(1982) Order without Government: The Society of the Pemon Indians of Venezuela, Urbana, IL: University of Illinois Press.Google Scholar
Thomas, L.
(1974) The Lives of a Cell: Notes of a Biology Watcher, NY: Bantam Books.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M.
(1996) “Do apes ape?”, in C. M. Heyes and B. G. Galef (eds) Social Learning: The Roots of Culture, NY: Academic.Google Scholar
(2009) Why we Cooperate, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. and J. Call
(1997) Primate Cognition, Oxford and NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., M. Carpenter, J. Call, T. Behne and H. Moll
(2005) “Understanding and sharing intensions: The origins of cultural cognition”, Brain and Behavior Science, 28. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., A. Melis, C. Tennie, E. Wyman and E. Herrmann
(2012) “Two key steps in the evolution of human cooperation”, Current Anthropology, 53. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. and A. Vaish
(2013) “Origins of human cooperation and morality”, Ann. Review of Psychology, 64. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tomlin, R.
(1995) “Focal attention, voice and word order”, in P. Downing and M. Noonan (eds) Word Order in Discourse, TSL #30, Amsterdam: J. Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1997) “Mapping conceptual representation into linguistic representation: The role of attention in grammar”, in J. Nuyts and E. Pederson (eds) Language and Conceptualization, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tooby, J. and L. Cosmides
(1992) “The psychological foundations of culture,” in J. H. Barkow, et al. (eds 1992) The Adapted Mind, Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Trabasso, T. and P. van den Broek
(1985) “Causal thinking and the representation of narrative events”, J. of Memory and Language, 24. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Traugott, E. C. and B. Heined
(eds 1991) Approaches to Grammaticalization, TSL #19.1,2, Amsterdam: J. Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Trivers, R.
(1971) “The evolution of reciprocal altruism”, Quarterly Review of Biology, 46. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tucker, D. M.
(1991) “Developing emotions and cortical networks”, in M. Gunnar and C. Nelson (eds), Developmental Neuroscience, Minnesota Symposium on Child Psychology, vol. 24, Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2001) “Motivated anatomy: A core-and-shell model of cortico-limbic architecture”, in F. Gainotti (ed.) Handbook of Neuropsychology, Amsterdam: Elsevier B.V.. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2002) “Embodied meaning: An evolutionary-developmental analysis of adaptive semantics”, in T. Givón and B. F. Malle (eds 2002) DOI logoGoogle Scholar
in preparation). Left Wing, Right Wing, University of Oregon, Eugene (ms.)
Tucker, D. M. and F. Luu
(2012) Cognition and Neural Development, Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Tucker, D. M., F. Luu and D. Derryberry
(2005) “Love hurts: The evolution of empathic concern through encephalization of nociceptive capacity”, Development and Psychopathology, 17. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tucker, D. M., D. L. Roth and T. B. Bair
(1986) “Functional connections among cortical regions: Topography of EEG Coherence”, Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 63:197–202. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tulvig, E. and W. Donaldson
(eds 1972) Organization of Memory, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Turnbull, C. M.
(1968) “Contemporary societies: The hunters,” in D. L. Sills (ed.) International Encyclopedia of the Social Sciences, Vol. 7, pp. 21–26, New York: Macmillan. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ungerleider, L. G. and M. Mishkin
(1982) “Two cortical visual systems”, in D. J. Ingle, R. J. W. Mansfield and M. A. Goodale (eds.) The Analysis of Visual Behavior, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press: 549–586. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Valentine, J. W.
(2004) On the Origin of Phyla, Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Vance, J. D.
(2016) Hillbilly Elegy, NY: Harper.Google Scholar
van de Kragt, A. J. C., R. M. Dawes and J. M. Orbell
(1988) “Are cooperators in social dilemma games ‘rational altruists’?”, Public Choice, 56. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
van Dijk, T. A. and W. Kintsch
(1983) Strategies of Discourse Comprehension, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
van Lawick-Goodall, J. and H. van Lawick
(1971) Innocent Killers, Boston: Houghton and Mifflin. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vendler, Z.
(1967) Linguistics and Philosophy, Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.Google Scholar
Venneman, T.
(1973) “Topics, subjects and word-order: From SXV to SVX via TVX”, paper read at the First International Congress on Historical Linguistics , Edinburgh, Sept. 1973 (ms.) DOI logo
Vincent, A.
(1973a) “Tairora verb structure”, in N. McKaughan (ed. 1973).Google Scholar
(1973b) “Notes on the Tairora noun morphology”, in N. McKaughan (ed. 1973) DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Waddington, C. H.
(1942) “Canalization of development and the inheritance of acquired characters”, Nature, 150. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1953) “”Genetic assimilation of an acquired character”, Evolution, 7. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Walker, C. H. and F. R. Yekovich
(1987) “Activation and use of script-based antecedents in anaphoric reference”, J. of Memory and Language, 26. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wallace, A. F. C.
(1961) Culture and Personality, New York: Random House. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Watson, J. D., and F. H. C. Crick
(1953) “A structure for deoxyribose nucleic acid”, Nature, 171. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Weiner, A.
(1976) Women of Value, Men of Renown: New Perspectives on Trobriand Exchange, Austin: University of Texas Press.Google Scholar
Wellman, H.
(1990) Children’s Theories of Mind, Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
West-Eberhard, M. J.
(1975) “The evolution of social behavior by kin selection”, Quarterly Review of Biology, 50.Google Scholar
(2003) Developmental Plasticity and Evolution, Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Whiten, A.
(ed. 1991) Natural Theories of Mind, Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
(2013) “Humans are not alone in computing how others see the world”, Animal Behavior, 86. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Whiten, A. and R. W. Byrne
(eds 1997) Machiavellian Intelligence II, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wilkinson, G. S.
(1990) “Food sharing in vampire bats”, American Scientist, (February) DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wilson, D. S.
(1975) “A theory of group selection”, Proc. of Nat. Acad., of Science, USA, 72. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1977) “Structured demes and the evolution of group advantageous traits”, American Naturalist, 111. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1980) The Natural Selection of Populations and Communities, Menlo Park, CA: Benjamin/Cummings.Google Scholar
(1997) “Altruism and organisms: Disentangling the themes of multi-level selection theory”, American Naturalist, 150. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wilson, E. O.
(1971) Insects Societies, Cambridge, MA: Harvar University Press.Google Scholar
(1975) Sociobiology: The New Synthesis, Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press (Belknap Press). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(ed. 2006) From so Simple Beginnings: The Four Great Books of Charles Darwin, NY: W.W. Norton & Co.. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2012) The Social Conquest of the Earth, NY: Liveright Publishing. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wilson, J. Q.
(1993) The Moral Sense, NY: Free Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wise, F.
(1994) “Spanish pre-grammatical baby talk”, Eugene: University of Oregon, Linguistics Dept. (ms.).Google Scholar
Wittgenstein, L.
(1918) Tractatus Logico Philosophicus, tr. by D. F. Pears and B. F. McGuiness, NY: The Humanities Press.Google Scholar
(1953) Philosophical Investigations, Tr. by G. E. M. Anscombe, NY: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Woodburn, J.
(1982) “Egalitarian societies”, Man, 17.3. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wrangham, R. and D. Peterson
(1996) Demonic Males: Apes and the Origin of Human Violence, Boston: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Wright, S. and T. Givón
(1987) “The pragmatics of indefinite reference”, Studies in Language, 11.1. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yekovich, F. R. and C. H. Walker
(1986) “The activation and use of scripted knowledge in reading about routine activities”, in B. K. Britton (ed.) Executive Control Processes in Reading, Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Young, P. and T. Givón
(1990b) “The Society of Intimates and the Society of Strangers”, Research Document no. xxxx, Institute of Cognitive and Decision Sciences, University of Oregon, Eugene, OR.Google Scholar
(2001) “Cooperation and manipulation in the Society of Intimates”, in M. Shibatani (ed.) The Grammar of Causation and Interpersonal Manipulation, TSL #48, Amsterdam: J. Benjamins.Google Scholar
Zuberbühler, K.
(2000) “Referential labeling in Diana monkeys”, Animal Behavior, 59. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2002) “A syntactic rule in forest monkey communications”, Animal Behavior, 63. DOI logoGoogle Scholar