Chapter published in:
Coherence
T. Givón
[Not in series 230] 2020
► pp. 104140
References

References

Barker, M.
(2003) “The representation of conversation in episodic memory: Information vs. interaction”, TR no. 03-1, Institute of Cognitive and Decision Sciences, University of Oregon, Eugene.Google Scholar
Barker, M. and T. Givón
(2002) “On the pre-linguistic origins of language processing rates”, in T. Givón and B. F. Malle (eds 2002).Google Scholar
Bentivoglio, P.
(1983) “Topic continuity and discontinuity in Discourse: A study in spoken Latin-American Spanish”, in T. Givón (ed. 1983).Google Scholar
Brown, C.
(1983) “Topic continuity in written English”, in T. Givón (ed. 1983).Google Scholar
Chafe, W.
(1994) Discourse, Consciousness and Time: Displacement of Conscious Experience in Speaking and Writing, Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
(1997) “Polyphonic topic development”, in T. Givón (ed. 1997).Google Scholar
Coates, J.
(1997) “The construction of collaborative floor in women’s friednly talk”, in Givón (ed. 1997).Google Scholar
Dickinson, C. and T. Givón
(1997) “”Memory and conversation: Toward and experimental paradigm”, in Givón (ed. 1997).Google Scholar
DuBois, J.
(1987) “The discourse basis of ergativity”, Language, 63.4.Google Scholar
Ericsson, A. and W. Kintsch
(1997) “Long term working memory”, Psychological Review, 102.2.Google Scholar
Ervin-Tripp, S. and A. Küntay
(1997) “Coherence in multi-party conversation: Episodes and contexts in interaction”, in T. Givón (ed. 1997).Google Scholar
Fox, A.
(1983) “Topic continuity in Biblical Hebrew”, in T. Givón (ed. 1983).Google Scholar
Gathercole, S. E. and A. D. Baddeley
(1993) Working Memory and Language, Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Gernsbacher, M. A.
(1990) Language Comprehension as Structure Building, Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
(ed. 1994) Handbook of Psycholinguistics, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Gernsbacher, M. A. and T. Givón
(eds 1994) Coherence in Spontaneous Discourse, TSL #31, Amsterdam: J. Benjamins.Google Scholar
Givón, T.
(1973) “Opacity and reference in language: An inquiry into the role of modalities”, in J. Kimball (ed.) Syntax and Semantics, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
(1977) “The drift from VSO to SVO in Biblical Hebrew: The pragmatics of Tense-Aspect”, in C. Li (ed. 1977) Mechanisms of Syntactic Change, Austin: University of Texas Press.Google Scholar
(1983a) “Topic continuity in discourse: Introduction”, in T. Givón (ed. 1983).Google Scholar
(1983b) “Topic continuity is spoken English” in T. Givón (ed. 1983).Google Scholar
(1983c) “Topic continuity in Ute”, in T. Givón (ed. 1983).Google Scholar
(ed. 1983) Topic Continuity in Discourse: Quantified Cross-Language Studies, TSL #3, Amsterdam: J. Benjamins.Google Scholar
(1984b) “Universals of discourse structure and second language acquisition”, in W. Rutherford (ed.) Language Universals and Second Language Acquisition, TSL #5, Amsterdam: J. Benjamins.Google Scholar
(1988) “The pragmatics of word-order: Predictability, importance and attention”, in E. Moranvcsik et al. (eds 1988).Google Scholar
(1991a) “Serial verbs and the mentral reality of ‘event’”, In E. Traugott and B. Heine (eds 1991, vol. I)Google Scholar
(1992) “The grammar of referential coherence as mental processing instructions”, Linguistics, 30.1.Google Scholar
(1995) Functionalism and Grammar, Amsterdam: J. Benjamins.Google Scholar
(ed. 1997) Conversation: Cognitive, Communicative and Social Perspectives, TSL #34, Amsterdam: J. Benjamins.Google Scholar
(2001) Syntax: An itroduction (2 vols.), Amsterdam: J. Benjamins.Google Scholar
(2002) Bio-Linguistics, Amsterdam: J. Benjamins.Google Scholar
(2005) Context as Other Minds, Amsterdam: J. Benjamins.Google Scholar
(2013) Ute Texts, Amsterdam: J. Benjamins.Google Scholar
(2015) The Diachrony of Grammar (2 vols.), Amsterdam: J. Benjamins.Google Scholar
Givón, T. and B. F. Malle
(eds 2002) The Evolution of Language out of Pre-Language, TSL #53, Amsterdam: J. Benjamins.Google Scholar
Givón, T., W. Kellog, P. Yee and M. I. Posner
(1985) “The identification of referents in connected discourse: Automatic vs. attended processing”, Cognitive Science Working Papers, Inst. for Cognitive and Decision Sciences, University of Oregon.Google Scholar
Gathercole, S. E. and A. D. Baddeley
(1993) Working Memory and Language, Hissdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Grimes, J.
(1975) The Thread of Discourse, The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Halliday, M. A. K. and R. Hassan
(1976) Cohesion in English, English Language Series, 9, London: Longman’s.Google Scholar
Hinds, J.
(1983) “Topic Continuity in Japanese”, in T. Givón (ed. 1983).Google Scholar
Johnson, N. S. and J. M. Mandler
(1977) “Remembrance of things parsed: Story structure and recall”, Cognitive Psychology, 9.Google Scholar
Kintsch, W.
(1982) “Memory for text”, in A. Flammer and W. Kintsch (eds) Text Processing, Amsterdam: North Holland.Google Scholar
(1992) “How readers construct situation models for stories: The role of syntactic cues and causal inference”, in A. F. Healy, S. Kosslyn and R. M. Shiffrin (eds) Essays in Honor of William K. Estes, Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
(1994) “The psychology of discourse processing”, in M. A. Gernsbacher (ed. 1994).Google Scholar
Kintsch, W. and T. van Dijk
(1978) “Toward a model of text comprehension and production”, Psychological Review, 85.Google Scholar
Linell, P. and N. Karolija
(1997) “Choosing the right quantifier: Usage in the context of communication”, in T. Givón (ed. 1997).Google Scholar
Longacre, R.
(1976) Anatomy of Speech Notions, Lisse: Peter de Ridder Press.Google Scholar
Mandler, J. M.
(1978) “A code in the node: The use of story schemata in retrieval”, Discourse Processes, 1.Google Scholar
Moravcsik, E., J. Wirth and M. xxx
(eds 1988) Typology and Language Universals, TSL #16, Amsterdam: J. Benjamins.Google Scholar
Neely, J. H.
(1990) “Semantic priming effects in visual word recognition: A selective review of current findings an theories”, in D. Besner and G. Humphreys (eds) Basic Research in Reading: Visual Word Recognition, Hillsdale, NJ: ERlbaum.Google Scholar
Ochs, E., B. Schieffelin and M. Platt
(1979) “Propositions across utterances and speakers”, in E. Ochs and B. Scieffelin (eds 1979).Google Scholar
Ochs, E. and B. Schieffelin
(eds 1979) Developmental Pragmatics, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Pu, M.-M.
(1997) “Zero anaphora and grammatical relations in Mandarin”, in T. Givón (ed. 1997b).Google Scholar
Traugott, E. and B. Heine
(eds 1991) Approaches to Grammaticalization, TSL #19:1,2, Amsterdam: J. Benjamins.Google Scholar
Quine, W. van O.
(1953) “Reference and Modality”, ch. 8 of From a Logical Point of View, Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Swinney, D. A.
(1979) “Lexical access during sentence comprehension: (Re)consideration of context effects”, J.V.L.V.B., 18.Google Scholar
Tomlin, R.
(1995) “Focal attention, voice and word order”, in P. Downing and M. Noonan (eds) Word Order in Discourse, TSL #30, Amsterdam: J. Benjamins.Google Scholar
(1997) “Mapping conceptual representation into linguistic representation: The role of attention in grammar”, in J. Nuyts and E. Pederson (eds) Language and Conceptualization, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Trabasso, T. and P. van den Broek
(1985) “Causal thinking and the representation of narrative events”, J. of Memory and Language, 24.Google Scholar
van Dijk, T. A. and W. Kintsch
(1983) Strategies of Discourse Comprehension, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Venneman, T.
(1973) “Topics, subjects and word-order: From SXV to SVX via TVX”, paper read at the First International Congress on Historical Linguistics, Edinburgh, Sept. 1973 (ms.)
Wright, S. and T. Givón
(1987) “The pragmatics of indefinite reference”, Studies in Language, 11.1.Google Scholar