Chapter published in:
Polylogues on The Mental Lexicon: An exploration of fundamental issues and directions
Edited by Gary Libben, Gonia Jarema and Victor Kuperman
[Not in series 238] 2021
► pp. 187220
References
Amenta, S., and Crepaldi, D.
(2012) Morphological processing as we know it: An analytical review of morphological efffecs in visual word identification. Frontiers in Psychology. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Baayen, R. H., Dijkstra, T., and Schreuder, R.
(1997) Singulars and Plurals in Dutch: Evidence for a Parallel Dual-Route Model, Journal of Memory and Language 37, 94–117. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Baayen, R. H., and Lieber, R.
(1991) Productivity and English derivation: a corpus-based study. Linguistics 29, 801–844. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A.
(1986) Working Memory. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Berwick, R., and Chomsky, N.
(2016) Why Only Us? Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bien, H., Baayen, R. H., and Levelt, W. J. M.
(2011) Frequency effects in the production of Dutch deverbal adjectives and inflected verbs. Language and Cognitive Processes 26, 683–715. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bock, K., and Loebell, H.
(1990) Framing sentences. Cognition 35, 1–39. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Booij, G.
(2010) Construction Morphology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
(2017) Inheritance and motivation in Construction Morphology. In Nikolas Gisborne and Andrew Hippisley (eds.), Default inheritance. Oxford: Oxford University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Butterworth, B.
(1983) Lexical representation. In B. Butterworth (ed.), Language Production: Vol. 2, 257–294. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bybee, J.
(1985) Morphology: a Study of the Relation between Meaning and Form. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(1995) Regular morphology and the lexicon. Language and Cognitive Processes 10: 425–455. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2001) Phonology and Language Use. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2010) Language, Usage and Cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, J., and D. Slobin
(1982) Rules and schemas in the development and use of the English past tense. Language 58: 265–289. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N.
(1965) Aspects of the Theory of Syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Clahsen, H.
(1999) Lexical entries and rules of language: A multidisciplinary study of German inflection. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22, 991–1060. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Collins, A., and Loftus, E.
(1975) A spreading-activation theory of semantic processing. Psychological Review 82, 407–428. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Collins, A., and Quillian, M.
(1969) Retrieval time from semantic memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 9, 240–247. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Culicover, P. W.
(1999) Syntactic Nuts. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Culicover, P. W., and Jackendoff, R.
(2005) Simpler Syntax. Oxford: Oxford University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Culicover, P. W., and Nowak, A.
(2003) Dynamical Grammar. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Daelemans, W., De Smet, K., and Gazdar, G.
(1992) Inheritance in natural language processing. Computational Linguistics 18, 205–218.Google Scholar
deJong, N.
(2002) Morphological Families in the Mental Lexicon. Nijmegen: Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics.Google Scholar
de Saussure, F.
(1916) Cours de linguistique générale, ed. C. Bally and A. Sechehaye, with the collaboration of A. Riedlinger, Lausanne and Paris: Payot; trans. W. Baskin, Course in General Linguistics, Glasgow: Fontana/Collins 1977.Google Scholar
Fiorentino, R. and Poeppel, D.
(2007) Compound words and structure in the lexicon. Language and Cognitive Processes 22, 953–1000. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fodor, J. D.
(1998) Learning to parse? In D. Swinney (ed.) Anniversary issue of Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 27.2, 285–318. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gallistel, C. R., and King, P. A.
(2009) Memory and the Computational Brain. Malden, MA; Wiley-Blackwell. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Goldberg, A.
(1995) Constructions: A Construction Grammar approach to argument structure, University of Chicago Press, Chicago.Google Scholar
(2005) Constructions at work, Oxford University Press, New York. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hagoort, P.
(2005) On Broca, brain, and binding: a new framework. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9, 416–423. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hale, J.
(2003) 2003. The Information Conveyed by Words in Sentences. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 32, 101–123. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2011) What a rational parser would do. Cognitive Science 35, 399–443. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Halle, M., and Marantz, A.
(1993) Distributed Morphology and the pieces of inflection. In K. Hale and S. J. Keyser (eds.), The View from Building 20, 111–176. Cambridge MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Hankamer, J.
(1989) Morphological parsing and the lexicon. In W. Marslen-Wilson (ed.), Lexical Representation and Process, 392–408. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Hay, J., and Baayen, H.
(2005) Shifting paradigms: Gradient structure in morphology, Trends in Cognitive Science 9.7, 342–348. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hoffman, T. and Trousdale, G.
(eds.) (2013) The Oxford Handbook of Construction Grammar. Oxford: Oxford University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Jackendoff, R.
(1975) Morphological and Semantic Regularities in the Lexicon, Language 51.3, 639–671. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2002) Foundations of Language. Oxford: Oxford University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2007) A Parallel Architecture Perspective on Language Processing, Brain Research 1146, 2–22. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2010) Meaning and the Lexicon. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Jackendoff, R. and Audring, J.
(2020) The Texture of the Lexicon: Relational Morphology and the Parallel Architecture. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kapatsinski, V.
(2007) Frequency, neighborhood density, age-of-acquisition, lexicon size, neighborhood density and speed of processing: Towards a domain-general, single-mechanism account. In S. Buescher, K. Holley, E. Ashworth, C. Beckner, B. Jones, and C. Shank. Proceedings of the 6th Annual High Desert Linguistics Society Conference, 121–40. Albuquerque, NM: High Desert Linguistics Society.Google Scholar
Kuperberg, G., and Jaeger, F.
(2016) What do we mean by prediction in language comprehension? Language, Cognition, and Neuroscience 31: 32–59. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kuperman, V., Bertram, R. and Baayen, R. H.
(2008) Morphological Dynamics in Compound Processing. Language and Cognitive Processes, 23, 1089–1132. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lakoff, G.
(1987) Women, Fire, and Dangerous Things. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Langacker, R.
(1987) Foundations of Cognitive Grammar, vol. 1, Stanford University Press, Stanford, CA.Google Scholar
Levelt, W., Roelofs, A., and Meyer, A.
(1999) A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22, 1–75. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Levy, R.
(2008) Expectation-Based Syntactic Comprehension. Cognition 106(3):1126–1177. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Libben, G.
(2006) Why study compounds? An overview of the issues. In G. Libben & G. Jarema, (Eds.), The representation and processing of compound words. Oxford: Oxford University Press (pp. 1–21).Google Scholar
Marcus, G.
(1998) Rethinking eliminative connectionism. Cognitive Psychology 37, 243–282. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2001) The Algebraic Mind. Cambridge MA: MIT Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Marr, D.
(1982) Vision. San Francisco: Freeman.Google Scholar
Marslen-Wilson, W., & Tyler, L. K.
(1980) The temporal structure of spoken language understanding. Cognition 8, 1–71. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Marslen-Wilson, W.
(1987) Functional parallelism in spoken language understanding. Cognition 25, 71–102. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Marslen-Wilson, W., and Zwitserlood, P.
(1989) Accessing spoken words: The importance of word onsets. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 15, 576–585.Google Scholar
Murphy, G.
(2002) The Big Book of Concepts. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Nooteboom, S., Weeman, F., and Wijnen, F.
(eds.) (2002) Storage and Computation in the Language Faculty. Dordrecht: Kluwer. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
O’Donnell, T. J.
(2015) Productivity and Reuse in Language: A Theory of Linguistic Computation and Storage. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Oldfield, R., and Wingfield, A.
(1965) Response latencies in naming objects. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 17, 273–281. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pinker, S.
(1999) Words and rules, Basic Books, New York.Google Scholar
Plag, I.
(2003) Word-Formation in English. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pollard, C. and Sag, I.
(1994) Head-driven Phrase Structure Grammar, University of Chicago Press, Chicago.Google Scholar
Radden, G., and Panther, K.-U.
(2004) Introduction: Reflections on motivation. In G. Radden and K.-U. Panther (eds.), Studies in Linguistic Motivation, 1–46. Berlin, New York: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Reifegerste, J.
(2014) Morphological Processing in Younger and Older People: Evidence for Flexible Dual-Route Access. Nijmegen: Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics.Google Scholar
Riehemann, S.
(1988) Type-based derivational morphology. Journal of Comparative Germanic Linguistics 2, 49–77. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rumelhart, D.
(1980) Schemata: The building blocks of cognition. In R. Spiro, B. Bruce, and W. Brewer, Theoretical Issues in Reading Comprehension: 33–58. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Rumelhart, D., and McClelland, J.
(1986) On learning the past tense of English verbs. In J. McClelland, D. Rumelhart, and the PDP Research Group, Parallel Distributed Processing, vol. ii, 216–71. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Schreuder, R., and R. H. Baayen
(1995) Modeling morphological processing. In L. Feldman (ed.), Morphological aspects of language processing, 131–154. Hove: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Siddiqi, D.
(2019) Distributed Morphology. In J. Audring and F. Masini (eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Morphological Theory. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 143–165.Google Scholar
Smolensky, P., and Legendre, G.
(2006) The Harmonic Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Swinney, D.
(1979) Lexical access during sentence comprehension: (Re)consideration of context effects. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 18, 645–659. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Taft, M.
(2004) Morphological decomposition and the reverse base frequency effect. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57A, 745–765. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tanenhaus, M., Leiman, J. M., Seidenberg, M.
(1979) Evidence for multiple stages in the processing of ambiguous words in syntactic contexts. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 18, 427–440. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M.
(2003) Constructing a Language: A Usage-Based Theory of Language Acquisition. Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Ullman, M. T.
(2015) The declarative/procedural model: A neurobiologically motivated theory of first and second language. In B. VanPatten and J. Williams (Eds.), Theories in second language acquisition: An introduction (2nd ed.). New York: Routledge. pp. 135–158.Google Scholar
Wittenberg, E. and Snedeker, J.
(2014) It takes two to kiss, but does it take three to give a kiss? Categorization based on thematic roles. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience 29.5, 635–641. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Woods, W.
(1980) Multiple theory formation in speech and reading. In R. Spiro, B. Bruce, and W. Brewer, Theoretical Issues in Reading Comprehension: 59–82. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Yang, Charles
(2016) The Price of Linguistic Productivity. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar